Recent studies show diabetes can be prevented. Growing knowledge of its biological bases opens further prevention opportunities. This article focuses on behavioral science research that may advance these opportunities. An ecological model guides attention to how prevention research may be pursued at the individual, group, or community levels. Three key areas are reviewed: risk communication, screening, and preventive interventions. Research on diabetes risk communication is limited but suggests that many are relatively unaware of risks and may have misconceptions about the disease. Amid policy debates and research regarding the potential benefits and costs of screening, identification of diabetes may itself be risky in terms of psychological and social consequences. The Diabetes Prevention Program and other studies make clear that diabetes can be prevented, both by the combination of weight loss and physical activity and by medications. Research needs to address promoting these methods to individuals as well as to groups and even whole communities. Fundamental as well as applied research should address how risks of diabetes are understood and may be communicated; how to enhance benefits and minimize psychological and other risks of screening; how to promote healthy eating and weight loss, physical activity, and appropriate use of medications to prevent diabetes; and how to reduce socioeconomic and cultural disparities in all these areas.

Preventing type 2 diabetes is now possible, as demonstrated by the Diabetes Prevention Program (DPP) (1) and other studies (2,3). Exciting developments in understanding the genetics and pathophysiology of diabetes will expand opportunities to prevent it. Behavioral science will be critical to the realization of these opportunities. As reviewed in this article, prevention of diabetes will benefit from advances in three key areas of behavior science: 1) how we conceptualize, assess, and communicate risk; 2) how we identify those at high risk; and 3) and how we reduce risk or prevent it in the first place, including among disadvantaged groups. This article is based on a group report from the November 1999 Conference on Behavioral Science Research in Diabetes sponsored by the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases (NIDDK).

The ecological model in Fig. 1 provides a framework for conceptualizing prevention. It outlines influences on behavior that operate within the individual, within family, and other groups and at a community level, including organizational, cultural, governmental, and policy influences. The model points to the importance of changes at multiple levels to encourage healthier communities (4). Taking up this approach, a recent report of the Institute of Medicine on promoting health stated one of two “overarching recommendations” as “interventions on social and behavioral factors should link multiple levels of influence (i.e., individual, interpersonal, institutional, community, and policy levels)” (5).

The ecological model has gained popularity in planning interventions, but it is also helpful in other areas, such as analysis of risk perception. For example, research may study differences in perceptions of risk among health care professionals, individuals with a disease, and the general public.

An estimated 15 million people in the U.S. have diabetes. Over 90% have type 2 diabetes. Pertinent to risk communication, it is estimated that at least one-third of individuals with type 2 diabetes do not know they have it, having not had appropriate screening and having no recognizable symptoms (6).

In addition to diabetes itself, impaired glucose tolerance (IGT) is a precursor to type 2 diabetes and is associated with increased risk of macrovascular disease. In addition to the 15 million people with diabetes, IGT has a prevalence of ∼11% in the U.S. population. The combined prevalence is 42% among older adults and is also heightened in ethnic minority groups (7).

The concept of risk does not apply only to the diagnosis. Diabetes is the leading cause of new cases of blindness in adults, the most common cause of end-stage renal disease, and the leading cause of nontraumatic amputations of lower limbs. Diabetes is associated with a severalfold increase in risk of cardiovascular disease. In summary, the risks associated with diabetes are enormous when diabetes is not controlled, prevented, or delayed.

In assessing risk and designing risk communications, it is helpful to distinguish among mental models, decision analysis, and risk communication.

Mental models of risk

Perceived risk reflects a variety of factors such as exposure and susceptibility to a risk, severity of consequences, the opportunities for control of the risk, and the certainty surrounding those estimates (8). Lay people bring to any risk a web of beliefs, called “mental models,” that reflect some mix of knowledge, misinformation, and ignorance. These mental models may be quite different than scientific models of well-understood phenomena. Individuals’ understanding of risk advances by integrating better information with their mental models. Mental models are critical to risk communication because, to be effective, communication needs to be understandable within an audience’s mental model.

A common approach to studying mental models compares a normative analysis (such as an “expert decision model” of what facts about a decision are most worth knowing) with a descriptive analysis of common mental models (what people currently believe) (9,10). The few studies that have compared expert and lay risk perceptions in diabetes indicate appreciable gaps (11,12). For example, relative to scientific estimates, Meltzer and Egleston (13) found that individuals with type 1 diabetes overestimated both their risk of complications and the benefits of intensive treatment.

Model building and specification allow development of communications to reduce gaps between mental and normative models (14). In diabetes, even identifying normative or expert models is challenging. Estimates of risks and benefits of risk reduction need to reflect multiple forms of morbidity as well as appreciable scientific uncertainty. Etiologies of both type 1 and type 2 diabetes are complex. Although consensus exists around the range of factors in the development of diabetes, there is uncertainty regarding their specific roles (6,15). Further complicating the public’s mental models is the existence of more than one type of diabetes. The absence of a well-articulated normative model and disagreements among experts provide additional sources of confusion to lay people.

In the context of the ecological model, mental models can be studied at the group or organizational/community levels as well as the individual level. From the individual perspective, research on personal models attempts to identify variables that the individual believes are central to living with diabetes (16). From the perspective of groups, some research has identified cultural variations in causal beliefs about diabetes as well as perceptions of severity (17). There is emerging evidence that people often misunderstand their own or their family member’s risk status. Certain ethnic and socioeconomic groups seem particularly vulnerable to such misunderstandings (18).

The general public and individuals with the disease may hold different mental models, with the public more conscious of acute problems (e.g., insulin shock) and those with the disease more concerned about chronic problems (19). This scenario may cause confusion regarding the seriousness of and susceptibility to diabetes or its complications.

Decision analysis

Decision analysis examines available choices and the full suite of issues that might impinge on a choice.This allows subsequent risk communication to focus on information most relevant to the available choices. Thus, for a decision about policies regarding weight loss, the decision analysis for a group living in a mild climate with easy access to healthy foods may be quite different than that for a group living in wintry isolation from a variety of fruits and vegetables.

Diabetes presents many risk management choices to affected individuals regarding lifestyle changes, type of treatment, revealing one’s health status, or insurance. Masking these decisions, onset of type 2 diabetes is often insidious so that millions remain without symptoms that might prompt diagnosis and explicit decision making (20).

Decisions about diabetes also include organizational decisions, such as about investments in lifestyle modification programs, and community or national policy decisions regarding screening programs, reimbursement for preventive services, regulation of the food supply, or provision of exercise resources (21).

Decisions in diabetes prevention draw on a common set of intellectual abilities, including understanding technical information, assessing uncertainties, anticipating one’s own future responses, and explaining one’s needs. Research compares how people perform such tasks relative to models of effective decision making. Framing decisions in terms that resonate with what people intuitively do facilitates better choices (2225).

Risk communication

Having compiled what is known about a problem, what people believe about it, what choices are available, and what factors impinge on those choices, developing communications can begin. Risk communications may be designed to facilitate risk management at the individual, group, or community/organization levels. At the latter, policy changes or economic, social, or cultural initiatives may be considered (26,27). Key considerations include 1) whether the goal is to persuade people to behave in a particular way or to inform them of their options; 2) whether the audience is composed of individuals, families and groups, organizations, communities, or policymakers; and 3) what forms of communication to use.

Risk communication needs to address both quantitative and qualitative components of mental models and decisions. On the one hand, individuals need accurate information about the probability and seriousness of the possible outcomes of their choices (e.g., a disease, a complication) (10,28,29). On the other hand, they need to understand the factors and processes that lead to these risks and that determine the quantitative estimates. In this area, findings of risk communication research may run counter to common sense. Conclusions stated in dogmatic or reassuring ways are not as effective as communications of clear, plausible mental models that justify those conclusions (9). When individuals understand risk estimates in the context of a coherent mental model, they feel competent acting on their beliefs as well as making sense of competing claims (e.g., in the news media, from friends). Insufficient confidence can lead to paralysis, when action is needed (26). But undue confidence can lead to ill-advised choices, when additional guidance should be sought.

Recommendation

Research should examine risk perception and decision making in individuals at risk of diabetes. Such research into their mental models needs to be framed by empirical models of key elements of diabetes risk, including policy analysis and judgments of seriousness, susceptibility, and treatment efficacy. Fundamental as well as applied behavioral research should elucidate how people comprehend and make wise decisions regarding the complexities of diabetes prevention. All of this may be facilitated by application to diabetes of research on risk communication in other areas of health and disease.

SCREENING FOR DIABETES

Screening recommendations

Screening for type 1 diabetes outside of well-defined research protocols is not recommended by the American Diabetes Association (ADA) because it is not clear what action should be taken if an at-risk individual is identified, the yield would be small because of low incidence, and some tests lack clear cutoff points (20).

Although we know that type 1 diabetes is a genetic and autoimmune disorder, the etiology of the disease remains unclear. Even among monozygotic twins, the concordance rate for type 1 diabetes is <50% (30). It is now believed that environmental factors trigger the autoimmune process in genetically at-risk individuals. Suspected environmental triggers range from purely biological (specific viruses) to behavioral (infant stress) (31). Behavioral science has an important role in determining behavioral and psychological triggers in the etiology of diabetes.

Although population screening for type 1 diabetes is not recommended, screening has an important role in studying individuals at risk to understand the natural history of the disease. Recognizing type 1 diabetes as an autoimmune disease led to screening programs to identify individuals with islet cell antibodies, indicating that the autoimmune process was underway (32,33). However, the trigger for the autoimmune process remains unknown. Consequently, genetic testing is currently being used to identify at-risk infants before inception of autoimmunity (34). Improved understanding of the pathogenesis of type 1 diabetes would offer hope of leading to effective preventive interventions.

For type 2 diabetes, the ADA (6,20) recommends that screening be performed within health care settings rather than community settings, which usually lack organized resources for follow-up of positive screening results. There are several options for type 2 diabetes screening. Fasting plasma glucose (FPG) is easier, more acceptable to screening candidates, and less expensive than, for example, the oral glucose tolerance test (OGTT). The OGTT is often used to follow up an abnormal or suspicious FPG. Other potential screening tests, such as HbA1c, are not currently recommended for diabetes screening.

Psychological factors in screening

Identification as “at risk” for type 1 diabetes is associated with psychological distress for both the individual at risk and for family members (3537). Although initial distress tends to dissipate over time, some individuals remain anxious some months after at-risk notification. Certain coping styles have been associated with persistence of anxiety (38). Screening of children raises concerns about the impact of at-risk identification on self-perceptions, social interactions in the developing child, and access to care. These risks are part of the reasoning behind recommendations that screening for type 1 diabetes be limited to well-defined research protocols (20).

Screening for type 2 diabetes appears to have varied effects: reassurance for some individuals and heightened anxiety for others. One study failed to show a rise in anxiety in a group of relatives of people with type 2 diabetes who themselves were being screened for the disease (39). In this study, people who reported prior treatment for anxiety or depression were more likely to be anxious after screening. Experiences with and reactions to screening may also vary by age and ethnicity (40). Little research has addressed the best methods for providing support or assistance during this process.

Recommendation

Research on psychological, behavioral, and social impacts of at-risk notification would inform further research on the best methods to communicate at-risk status and to help individuals and families cope with at-risk notification. Both early identification programs and research to improve them would benefit from research on psychological factors related to subject recruitment and retention in natural history and prevention trials. Reflecting the reciprocal nature of research in all of these areas, research on behavioral or psychological factors in disease etiology may expand the base of knowledge for identification of those at risk.

LIFESTYLE AND MEDICATION IN PREVENTION OF TYPE 2 DIABETES

Recent studies in China (2), Finland (3), and the U.S. have demonstrated that diabetes can be prevented. The DPP (1) evaluated lifestyle intervention and metformin use in preventing diabetes in 3,234 adults between the ages of 25 and 85 years and with IGT, treated in 27 centers throughout the U.S. The lifestyle intervention focused on loss of 7% of body weight and on achieving 150 min physical activity each week. The intervention was administered by an individual coach through a series of 16 sessions during the first 24 weeks of the program. These sessions were followed by individual contact at least monthly and face-to-face contact at least bimonthly for the balance of the DPP. The intervention included behavioral management procedures, such as goal setting, identification of concrete change goals, and identification of plans for coping with temptations for relapse, all implemented in a highly individualized manner (41).

The DPP also included a double-blinded placebo-controlled test of metformin, a medication long used in the management of type 2 diabetes. The metformin and placebo groups also received encouragement to lose weight and exercise, repeated yearly but without individualized discussion of how to achieve these goals (41).

The results of the DPP were extremely encouraging. Relative to placebo, the lifestyle intervention reduced the incidence of diabetes by 58%, and metformin reduced the incidence by 31%. Countering expectations that “old dogs” cannot learn “new tricks,” the lifestyle intervention reduced the incidence of diabetes by 71% among those 60 years of age or older, a group at heightened risk and burden, in contrast to a 58% reduction for the entire age range (1).

The Finnish Diabetes Prevention Study also found a 58% reduction in incidence of diabetes among high-risk individuals receiving a lifestyle intervention (3). A kind of dose/response analysis identified an additive benefit of each of five changes emphasized: fiber consumption, fruit and vegetable consumption, reduced dietary fat, exercise, and weight loss. The more targets that participants were able to meet, the lower their likelihood of converting to diabetes.

The results of the DPP illustrate how decision analysis can depend on the ecological level at which risks are considered. Aggregated over an entire population, the difference between the lifestyle intervention and metformin arms (58 vs. 31% reduction in incidence) is enormous (1). This difference might justify broad policies to promote lifestyle changes to prevent diabetes. But considered from the perspective of the individual with IGT, metformin reduces the 11% per year risk of converting to diabetes by 31% to 7.8%. Lifestyle reduces it by 58% to 4.8% (1). Therefore, the individual may choose between the daily effort of lifestyle modification, with a 4.8% yearly risk of conversion, and the lesser effort of taking a pill, with a 7.8% yearly risk. The advantages of lifestyle intervention over metformin may be less clear to the individual than the community.

Prevention through lifestyle changes

The results of the DPP point toward the importance of research on weight loss, dietary change, and physical activity. Reviewing the entirety of this literature is not possible within the confines of this article. Fortunately, recent comprehensive reviews address each. A 1998 report on overweight and obesity of the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute (NHLBI) and the NIDDK concluded that “a combined intervention of behavior therapy (such as stimulus control, problem solving), and LCD (low-calorie diet), and increased physical activity provides the most successful therapy for weight loss and weight maintenance” (42). (The report recommended pharmacological agents only for individuals with a BMI ≥30 kg/m2 or, if other medical conditions indicate urgency of weight loss, between 27 and 30 kg/m2.)

Promotion of physical activity has recently been reviewed by the Task Force on Community Preventive Services organized by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (43), and promotion of a healthy diet is the focus of a recent review of the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) (44). In addition to the endorsement of behavior therapy and behavior change strategies of the NHLBI/NIDDK obesity report, both of these reports also conclude that effective program characteristics include comprehensively addressing behavior change through as many channels and levels (individual, group, community) as possible and sustaining interventions over as long a period as possible.

A recent article from another of the panels of the November 1999 Conference on Behavioral Science Research in Diabetes reported on weight loss with clinical populations or in relatively intensive interventions delivered to individuals or through small groups (45). Complementing that article, the AHRQ report on dietary change focused on interventions directed toward general populations. The following paragraphs summarize findings of this AHRQ Diet Report, supplemented by selected additional citations. This summary should provide a sampling of characteristics of successful programs for lifestyle change at the individual, organizational, and community levels.

Interventions directed toward individuals.

The lifestyle intervention in the DPP was administered primarily one-on-one and in a highly individualized and flexible manner. The average weight loss in the first year of the intervention approximated the target of 7% body weight, or ∼15 lbs (41). Across the several years of the DPP, a sustained weight loss totaling 5% of body weight was achieved.

As documented in the AHRQ Diet Report, most dietary interventions directed to individuals have focused on dietary fat with lesser emphasis on fiber, fruits, and vegetables or weight loss. Most have been implemented through health care settings. Strategies that have achieved significant reductions in fat intake have included group counseling; inclusion of family and spouses; focus on behavioral approaches such as self-monitoring, problem solving, and relapse prevention (46,47); emphasis on individualizing interventions according to stages of change (4850); and use of computer or interactive video components (51).

Interventions through organizations.

In general, school-based interventions that have been successful have focused on multiple levels of intervention, including classroom instruction by teachers, environmental change such as cafeteria food choices, and family involvement through dietary-related homework, activity packets, or group meetings (5254). The CATCH (Child and Adolescent Trial for Cardiovascular Health) trial demonstrated that altering school lunch and physical education environments can influence dietary behaviors of children (5557).

Organizational settings have also included work sites. Successful programs most often used multiple strategies across multiple levels, such as including workers’ families (58) or environmental changes such as increasing availability of healthy food choices (59). Tactics in successful programs include the following: screenings, nutrition classes, goal setting, changes in food available through cafeterias and vending machines, individualized feedback on food intake, mailed self-help materials, family components, and participatory approaches such as employee advisory boards to help plan interventions (5860). Focusing on naturally occurring groups, one successful work site program was organized around informal cliques of blue-collar employees (61).

Interventions at the community level.

As with programs implemented through schools and work sites, successful community programs have been comprehensive. Specific tactics in successful programs have included attention to self-management strategies, family components (62), emphasis on peers or lay instructors (6366), the combination of home visits and newsletters (67), community-based classes (68), altering supermarket environments (69), and combining channels such as reaching adolescent women through Girl Scout troops and then using them to reach their parents (70).

Among major community-based cardiovascular disease prevention programs, the Stanford Five City Study used intensive instruction on diet and an extensive media campaign (7173). Results showed significant decreases in saturated fat intake. The Minnesota Heart Health program and Project LEAN (Low Fat Eating for America Now) used public/private partnerships to enhance delivery of dietary campaign messages (74). Most successful in changing diet and other risk factors and reducing cardiovascular mortality, the North Karelia project used probably the broadest range of interventions, from mass media to cooperation with agricultural and food merchandising groups, in order to improve the availability of healthy foods such as low-fat milk (75).

Pharmacologic prevention interventions and adherence

With increasing understanding of the pathophysiology of type 2 diabetes and the possibility of identifying genotypes indicative of specific vulnerabilities, opportunities for pharmacological interventions to prevent diabetes will grow. Metformin, used in the DPP, is a biguanide antihyperglycemic agent used for >40 years in many countries but only approved in the U.S. in 1996 (41).

Adherence was critical to the success of metformin. This was especially a challenge because of metformin’s gastrointestinal side effects for some individuals. In quarterly structured interviews, DPP medication case managers used motivational interviewing techniques, a problem-solving format, and varied tactics from a medication “toolbox” to individualize plans for improving adherence. Results were impressive. Among individuals taking the drug, 72% took ≥80% of prescribed medication. Among those taking placebo, 77% achieved this level of adherence. The full dose of 850 mg metformin twice daily was prescribed to 84% of subjects randomized to the active metformin arm, whereas the remaining 16% presumably had side effects that limited them to 850 mg once daily (1).

Poor adherence to preventive and therapeutic medications is common (76). Belief in the efficacy of the medication, simplicity of the regimen, and its ability to be integrated into daily routines encourage adherence. Cost and adverse side effects discourage adherence (77). Accordingly, behavioral interventions have succeeded by addressing forgetting to take medications, fear of taking medications, lack of a routine for taking medications, and limited access to medications during a busy schedule. Support for adherence is also important from family and friends (77). Age, ethnicity, mental status, sex, and other demographic variables require further study regarding their roles in adherence to a medication to prevent chronic disease. Research in adherence is complicated by reporting biases and errors in measurement of pill taking (78).

Problems specific to adherence to preventive medications include absence of symptoms that might prompt pill taking, uncertainty about the need for the medication (especially when there are no symptoms for it to relieve), and barriers related to health beliefs for preventive versus therapeutic remedies. These problems underscore the earlier discussion of the importance of a clear mental model of risk as a basis for risk communication and decisions among alternative actions.

Other risky behaviors

Many behaviors other than those considered here contribute to disease burden among people with diabetes. For example, smoking is not a risk factor for diabetes, but the synergy of smoking and diabetes entails as much as a 12-fold increase in cardiovascular risk (79). Comprehensive efforts to prevent diabetes should include attention to smoking and other behavior patterns such as stress or depression (80), which are often associated with diabetes and add to its burden.

Recommendation

A major priority is translation of DPP findings that type 2 diabetes can be prevented. This will benefit from progress in risk communication, early identification of those at risk, and participatory research addressing cultural, social, and community factors in diabetes prevention. Research should examine strengths and limitations of informal community channels and participatory approaches, formal professional channels, and technological approaches (e.g., computer-based tailored communications) to population-based health communication and prevention. In addition to diet, weight, and physical activity, prevention research should include smoking and other behaviors that contribute to the disease burden of diabetes.

Research on mental models and decisions about taking preventive medications should facilitate translation of the DPP’s positive results with metformin. With this as a background, research should examine risk communications and multiple brief behavioral interventions from the DPP to promote use of and adherence to medications for diabetes prevention.

SOCIOCULTURAL DISPARITIES

Diabetes is sharply stratified by socioeconomic disparities, with heightened prevalence and burden among people of color and those who are educationally or economically disadvantaged (8183). Thus, an important feature of the DPP was its inclusion of African-Americans, American Indians, Asian Americans, and Hispanics and Latinos who, together, made up 45% of participants—more than in any other major clinical or prevention trial. Most impressive, the striking benefits of lifestyle intervention and metformin did not differ either among or between the minority groups and the other 55% of participants (1).

Two key observations about reaching disadvantaged minorities are that they are relatively isolated from formal or mainstream channels of information and that they use informal sources of information (84). Both observations suggest peer-based and community programs that will enlist informal social networks to disseminate health messages and provide peer and informal support for behavior change (85).

One approach to this is to implement programs through existing networks of intended audiences. For example, Stolley and Fitzgibbon (86) implemented an intervention promoting low-fat diet through a tutoring program for low-income mothers and daughters. Similarly, Hispanic families were reached through literacy training programs (62).

Church-based approaches have been popular in efforts to reach African-Americans. A church- and community-based program promoted healthy eating through lay health advisors, pastor support, community coalitions, and distribution of materials through local grocers (87). In Samoa, a church-based program used a participatory approach and was successful in reducing waist circumference and eliminating weight gain among those at high risk for diabetes (88).

Recommendation

Research should examine how risk perception and communication, early identification, and prevention are influenced by group and cultural differences and social and economic factors associated with disproportionate diabetes burden. Research to reduce disproportionate diabetes burden should include participatory approaches to interventions.

GENERAL THEMES AND CONCLUSIONS

Progress in any one of risk communication, early detection, and prevention creates opportunities for progress in each of the other areas. For example, better understanding of diabetes and approaches to early detection identify new topics for risk communication and prevention. At the same time, improved prevention programs increase the range of alternatives for risk communication to address and increase the potential benefits of early detection. Similarly, better approaches to risk communication may increase the numbers interested in early identification and prevention. Finally, all of these will be enhanced by better understanding of the etiology of diabetes.

Fundamental research

Cutting across all the topics addressed is the utility of fundamental research to increase understanding of how people recognize and appraise risks; how they respond to early identification of their own risks; how to encourage healthy diet, weight loss, exercise, and appropriate use of medications; and how all of these may be influenced by social and cultural differences.

Figure 1—

Ecological model of health behavior.

Figure 1—

Ecological model of health behavior.

Close modal
1
Diabetes Prevention Program Research Group: Reduction in the incidence of type 2 diabetes with lifestyle intervention or metformin.
N Engl J Med
. 
346
:
393
–403,
2002
2
Pan XR, Li GW, Hu YH, Wang JX, Yang WY, An ZX, Hu ZX, Lin J, Xiao JZ, Cao HB, Liu PA, Jiang XG, Jiang YY, Wang JP, Zheng H, Zhang H, Bennett PH, Howard BV: Effects of diet and exercise in preventing NIDDM in people with impaired glucose tolerane: the Da Qing IGT and Diabetes Study.
Diabetes Care
20
:
537
–544,
1997
3
Tuomilehto J, Lindstrom J, Eriksson JG, Valle TT, Hamalainen H, Ilanne-Parikka P, Keinanen-Kiukaanniemi S, Laakso M, Louheranta A, Rastas M, Salminen V, Uusitupa M: Prevention of type 2 diabetes mellitus by changes in lifestyle among subjects with impaired glucose tolerance.
N Engl J Med
344
:
1343
–1350,
2001
4
Stokols D: Translating social ecological theory into guidelines for community health promotion.
Am J Health Promot
10
:
282
–298,
1996
5
Smedley BD, Syme SL:
Promoting Health: Intervention Strategies from Social and Behavioral Research
. Washington, DC, National Academy Press,
2000
6
Engelgau M, Narayan KMV, Herman WH: Screening for type 2 diabetes (Technical Review).
Diabetes Care
23
:
1563
–1580,
2000
7
Harris MI: Impaired glucose tolerance in the U.S. population.
Diabetes Care
12
:
464
–474,
1989
8
Slovic P:
Perception of Ris
. London, Earthscan,
2001
9
Bostrom A, Fischhoff B, Morgan MG: Characterizing mental models of hazardous processes: a methodology and an application to radon.
J Soc Issues
48
:
85
–100,
1992
10
Fischhoff B: Why (cancer) risk communication can be hard.
J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr
25
:
7
–13,
1999b
11
Walker EA, Fisher EB, Marrero DG, McNabb W: Comparative risk judgments among participants in the Diabetes Prevention Program (Abstract).
Diabetes
50 (Suppl. 1)
:
A397
,
2001
12
Walker EA, Flynn J, Wylie-Rosett J, Mertz CK, Kalten M: Perception of risk for developing diabetes among physicians (Abstract).
Diabetes
48 (Suppl. 1)
:
A321
,
1999
13
Meltzer D, Egleston B: How patients with diabetes perceive their risk of major complications.
Eff Clin Pract
3
:
7
–15,
2000
14
Morgan MG, Fischhoff B, Bostrom A, Altman C:
Risk Communication: The Mental Models Approac
. New York, Cambridge University Press,
2001
15
Griffin SJ, Little PS, Hales CN, Kinmonth AL, Wareham NJ: Diabetes risk score: toward earlier detection of type 2 diabetes in general practice.
Diabetes Metab Res Rev
16
:
164
–171,
2000
16
Skinner TC, Hampson SE: Personal models of diabetes in relation to self-care, well-being, and glycemic control.
Diabetes Care
24
:
828
–833,
2001
17
Greenhalgh T, Helman C, Chowdhury A: Health beliefs and folk models of diabetes in British Bangladeshis: a qualitative study.
BMJ
316
:
978
–983,
1998
18
Johnson SB: Screening programs to identify children at risk for diabetes mellitus: psychological impact on children and parents.
J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab
14
:
653
–659,
2001
19
Walker EA, Basch CE, Howard CJ, Kromholz WN, Zybert PA, Shamoon H: Incentives and barriers to retinopathy screening in African Americans with diabetes.
J. Diabetes Complications
11
:
298
–306,
1997
20
American Diabetes Association: Screening for diabetes (Position Statement).
Diabetes Care
24 (Suppl. 1)
:
S21
–S24,
2001
21
Brownson CA, Dean C, Dabney S, Brownson RC: Cardiovascular risk reduction in rural minority communities: the Bootheel Heart Health Project.
J Health Educ
29
:
158
–165,
1998
22
Fischhoff B: Judgment and decision making. In
The Psychology of Human Thought
. Sternberg RJ, Smith EE, Eds. New York, Cambridge University Press,
1988
, p.
153
–187
23
Hastie R, Dawes RD:
Rational Choice in an Uncertain Worl
. San Diego, CA, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich,
2001
24
Plous S:
The Psychology of Judgment and Decision Makin
. Hightstown, NY, McGraw-Hill,
1993
25
Yates JF:
Judgment and Decision Makin
. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall,
1990
26
Fischhoff B: Giving advice: decision theory perspectives on sexual assault.
Am Psychol
47
:
577
–588,
1992
27
Fischhoff B, Downs J, Bruine de Bruin W: Adolescent vulnerability: a framework for behavioral interventions.
Appl Prev Psychol
7
:
77
–94,
1998
28
Fischhoff B: What do patients want? Help in making effective choices.
Eff Clin Pract
2
:
198
–200,
1999
29
Fischhoff B, Bostrom A, Quadrel MJ: Risk perception and communication. In
Oxford Textbook of Public Health
. Detels R, McEwen J, Omenn G, Eds. London, Oxford University Press,
1997
, p.
987
–1002
30
Olmos P, A’Hern R, Heaton DA, Millward BA, Risley D, Pyke DA, Leslie RD: The significance of concordance rate for type 1 (insulin dependent) diabetes in identical twins.
Diabetologia
31
:
747
–750,
1988
31
Dorman J, McCarthy B, O’Leary L, Koehler A: Risk factors for insulin-dependent diabetes. In
Diabetes in America
. 2nd ed. Harris M, Ed.
1995
, p.
165
–178 (NIH publ. no. 95-1468)
32
Riley WJ, Maclaren NK, Krisher J, Spillar RP, Silverstein JH, Schatz DA, Schwartz S, Malone J, Shah S, Vadheim C, Roter JI: A prospective study of the development of diabetes in relatives of patients with insulin-dependent diabetes.
N Engl J Med
323
:
1167
–1172,
1990
33
Schatz D, Krisher J, Horne G, Rile W, Spillar R, Silverstein J, Winter W, Muir A, Derovanesian D, Shah S, Malone J, Maclaren N: Islet cell antibodies predict insulin-dependent diabetes in the United States school age children as powerfully as in unaffected relatives.
J Clin Invest
93
:
2403
–2407,
1994
34
Schatz D, Muir A, Fuller K, Atkinson M, Crockett S, Hsiang H, Winter W, Ellis T, Taylor K, Saites C, Dukes M, Fange Q, Clare-Salzler M, She J: Prospective Assessment in Newborns for Diabetes Autoimmunity (PANDA): a newborn diabetes screening program in the general population in Florida (Abstract).
Diabetes
49 (Suppl. 1)
:
A67
,
2000
35
Galatzer A, Green E, Ofan R, Benzaquen H, Yosefsberg Z, Weintrob N, Karp M, Vardi P: Psychological impact of islet cell antibody screening.
J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab
14
:
675
–679,
2001
36
Johnson SB, Riley WJ, Hansen CA, Nurick MA: Psychological impact of islet cell-antibody screening: preliminary results.
Diabetes Care
13
:
93
–97,
1990
37
Johnson SB, Tercyak KP: Psychological impact of islet cell antibody screening for IDDM on children, adults, and their family members.
Diabetes Care
18
:
1370
–1372,
1995
38
Johnson SB, Carmichael SK: At-risk for diabetes: coping with the news.
J Clin Psychol Med Settings
7
:
69
–78,
2000
39
Farmer AJ, Levy J, Turner RC: Perceptions of risk of diabetes and anxiety following screening for type 2 diabetes (Abstract).
Diabetes
48 (Suppl. 1)
:
A321
,
1999
40
Bastian H, Keirse MJNC, Searle J:
Influencing People’s Experiences of Screenin
. Oxford, U.K., The Cochrane Library I edition, Update Software,
2000
41
Diabetes Prevention Program Research Group: The Diabetes Prevention Program: design and methods for a clinical trial in the prevention of type 2 diabetes.
Diabetes Care
22
:
623
–634,
1999
42
National Heart Lung and Blood Institute and National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Disease:
Clinical Guidelines on the Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adult
. Washington, DC, National Institutes of Health,
1998
43
Task Force on Community Preventive Services.
Increasing Physical Activity: A Report on Recommendations of the Task Force on Community Preventive Services
. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Reports Recommendations and Reports 2001. Vol. 50, no. RR-18, Centers for Disease Control,
2001
44
Agency for Healthcare Research and Policy:
The Efficacy of Interventions to Modify Dietary Behavior Related to Cancer Ris
. AMRQ, Rockville, MD,
2001
45
Wing RR, Goldstein MG, Acton KJ, Birch LL, Jakicic JM, Sallis JF Jr, Smith-West D, Jeffery RW, Surwit RS: Behavioral science research in diabetes: lifestyle changes related to obesity, eating behavior, and physical activity.
Diabetes Care
24
:
117
–123,
2001
46
Glasgow RE, Toobert DJ, Mitchell DL, Donnelly JE, Calder D: Nutrition education and social learning interventions for type II diabetes.
Diabetes Care
12
:
150
–152,
1989
47
Beresford SAA, Curry SJ, Kristal AR, Lazovich D, Feng Z, Wagner EH: A dietary intervention in primary care practice: the eating patterns study.
Am J Public Health
87
:
610
–616,
1997
48
Kristal AR, Curry SJ, Shattuck AL, Feng Z, Li S: A randomized trial of a tailored, self-help dietary intervention: the Puget Sound Eating Patterns study.
Prev Med
31
:
380
–389,
2000
49
Marcus AC, Morra M, Rimer BK, Stricker M, Heimendinger J, Wolfe P, Darrow SL, Hamilton L, Cox DS, Miller N, Perocchia RS: A feasibility test of a brief educational intervention to increase fruit and vegetable consumption among callers to the Cancer Information Service.
Prev Med
27
:
250
–261,
1998
50
Marcus AC, Heimendinger J, Wolfe P, Rimer BK, Morra M, Cox D, Lang PJ, Stengle W, Van Herle MP, Wagner D, Fairclough D, Hamilton L: Increasing fruit and vegetable consumption among callers to the CIS: results from a randomized trial.
Prev Med
27
:
S16
–S28,
1998
51
Glasgow RE, Toobert DJ, Hampson SE: Effects of a brief office-based intervention to facilitate diabetes dietary self-management.
Diabetes Care
19
:
835
–842,
1996
52
Domel SB, Baranowski T, Davis H, et al: Development and evaluation of a school intervention to increase fruit and vegetable consumption among 4th and 5th grade students.
J Nutr Educ
25
:
345
–349,
1993
53
Luepker RV, Perry CL, McKinlay SM, Nader PR, Parcel GS, Stone EJ, Webber LS, Elder JP, Feldman HA, Johnson CC, et al: Outcomes of a field trial to improve children’s dietary patterns and physical activity: the Child and Adolescent Trial for Cardiovascular Health (CATCH).
JAMA
275
:
768
–776,
1996
54
Perry CL, Bishop DB, Taylor G, Murray DM, Mays RW, Dudovitz BS, Smyth M, Story M: Changing fruit and vegetable consumption among children: the 5-a-Day Power Plus Program in St. Paul, Minnesota.
Am J Public Health
88
:
603
–609,
1998
55
Nader PR, Stone EJ, Lytle LA, Perry CL, Osganian SK, Kelder S, Webber LS, Elder JP, Montgomery D, Feldman HA, Wu M, Johnson C, Parcel GS, Luepker RV: Three-year maintenance of improved diet and physical activity: the CATCH cohort: Child and Adolescent Trial for Cardiovascular Health.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med
153
:
695
–704,
1999
56
Perry CL, Lytle LA, Feldman H, et al: Effects of the Child and Adolescent Trial for Cardiovascular Health (CATCH) on fruit and vegetable intake.
J Nutr Educ
30
:
354
–360,
1998
57
Stone EJ, Osganian SK, McKinlay SM, Wu WC, Webber LS: Operational design and quality control in the CATCH multicenter trial.
Prev Med
25
:
384
–399,
1996
58
Tilley BC, Glanz K, Kristal AR, Hirst K, Li S, Vernon SW, Myers R: Nutrition intervention for high-risk auto workers: results of the Next Step Trial.
Prev Med
28
:
284
–292,
1999
59
Sorensen G, Thompson B, Glanz K, Feng Z, Kinne S, DiClemente C, Emmons K, Heimendinger J, Probart C, Lichtenstein E: Work site-based cancer prevention: primary results from the Working Well Trial.
Am J Public Health
86
:
939
–947,
1996
60
Sorensen G, Morris DM, Hunt MK, Hebert JR, Harris DR, Stoddard A, Ockene JK: Work-site nutrition intervention and employees’ dietary habits: the Treatwell program.
Am J Public Health
82
:
877
–880,
1992
61
Buller DB, Morrill C, Taren D, Aickin M, Sennott-Miller L, Buller MK, Larkey L, Alatorre C, Wentzel TM: Randomized trial testing the effect of peer education at increasing fruit and vegetable intake.
J Natl Cancer Inst
91
:
1491
–1500,
1999
62
Fitzgibbon ML, Stolley MR, Avellone ME, Sugerman S, Chavez N: Involving parents in cancer risk reduction: a program for Hispanic American families.
Health Psychol
15
:
413
–422,
1996
63
Havas S, Anliker J, Damron D, Langenberg P, Ballesteros M, Feldman R: Final results of the Maryland WIC 5-A-Day Promotion Program.
Am J Public Health
88
:
1161
–1167,
1998
64
Havas S, Treiman K, Langenberg P, Ballesteros M, Anliker J, Damron D, Feldman R: Factors associated with fruit and vegetable consumption among women participating in WIC.
J Am Diet Assoc
98
:
1141
–1148,
1998
65
Auslander W, Haire-Joshu D, Williams JH, Houston C, Krebill H: The short-term impact of a health promotion program for African-American women.
Res Soc Work Pract
10
:
78
–97,
2000
66
Haire-Joshu D, Brownson R, Schechtman K, Nanney S, Houston C, Auslander W: A community research partnership to improve the diet of African Americans.
Am J Health Behav
25
:
140
–146,
2001
67
Knutsen SF, Knutsen R: The Tromso Survey: the Family Intervention study: the effect of intervention on some coronary risk factors and dietary habits, a 6-year follow-up.
Prev Med
20
:
197
–212,
1991
68
Hartman TJ, McCarthy PR, Park RJ, Schuster E, Kushi LH: Results of a community-based low-literacy nutrition education program.
J Community Health
22
:
325
–341,
1997
69
Rodgers AB, Kessler LG, Portnoy B, Potosky AL, Patterson B, Tenney J, Thompson FE, Krebs-Smith SM, Breen N, Mathews O, et al: “Eat for Health”: a supermarket intervention for nutrition and cancer risk reduction.
Am J Public Health
84
:
72
–76,
1994
70
Cullen KW, Bartholomew LL, Parcel GS: Girl scouting: an effective channel for nutrition education.
J Nutr Educ
29
:
86
–91,
1997
71
Farquhar JW, Fortmann SP, Flora JA, Taylor CB, Haskell WL, Williams PT, Macoby N, Woods PD: Effects of community-wide education on cardiovascular disease risk factors: the Stanford Five-City Project.
JAMA
264
:
359
–365,
1990
72
Farquhar JW, Fortmann SP, Maccoby N, Woods PD, Haskell WL, Taylor CB, Flora JA, Solomon DS, Togers T, Adler E, Breitrose P, Weiner L: The Stanford Five City Project: an overview. In
Behavioral Health: A Handbook of Health Enhancement and Disease Prevention
. Matarazzo JD, Weiss SM, Herd JA, Miller NE, Weiss SM, Eds. New York, Wiley,
1984
, p.
1154
–1165
73
Fortmann SP, Winkleby MA, Flora JA, Haskell WL, Taylor CB: Effect of long-term community health education on blood pressure and hypertension control: the Stanford Five-City Project.
Am J Epidemiol
132
:
629
–646,
1990
74
Heimendinger J, Van Duyn MA, Chapelski D, Foerster S, Stables G: The National 5 a Day for Better Health Program: a large-scale nutrition intervention.
J Public Health Manage Pract
2
:
27
–35,
1996
75
Puska P, Nissinen A, Tuomilehto J, Salonen JT, Koskela K, McAlister A, Kottke TE, Maccoby N, Farquhar JW: The community-based strategy to prevent coronary heart disease: conclusions from the ten years of the North Karelia Project.
Annu Rev Public Health
6
:
147
–193,
1985
76
Haynes RB, McKibbon KA, Kanoni R: Systematic review of randomised trials of interventions to assist patients to follow prescriptions for medications.
Lancet
348
:
383
–386,
1996
77
Dunbar-Jacob J: Contributions to patient adherence: is it time to share the blame.
Health Psychol
12
:
91
–92,
1993
78
Liu H, Golin C, Miller LG, Hays RD, Beck CK, Sanandaji S, Christian J, Maldonado T, Duran D, Kaplan AH, Wenger NS: A comparison study of multiple measures of adherence to HIV protease inhibitors.
Ann Intern Med
134
:
968
–977,
2001
79
Haire-Joshu D, Glasgow R, Tibbs T: Smoking and diabetes.
Diabetes Care
22
:
1887
–1898,
1999
80
Lustman PJ, Anderson RJ, Freedland KE, de Groot ME, Carney RM, Clouse RE: Depression and poor glycemic control: a meta-analytic review of the literature.
Diabetes Care
23
:
934
–942,
2000
81
Harris MI, Eastman RC, Cowie CC, Flegal KM, Eberhardt MS: Racial and ethnic differences in glycemic control of adults with type 2 diabetes.
Diabetes Care
22
:
403
–403,
1999
82
Carter JS, Pugh JA, Monterrosa A: Non-insulin dependent diabetes mellitus in minorities in the United States.
Ann Intern Med
125
:
221
–232,
1996
83
Harris MI: Racial and ethnic differences in health care access and health outcomes for adults with type 2 diabetes.
Diabetes Care
24
:
454
–459,
2001
84
Dressler WW:
Stress and Adaptation in the Context of Cultur
. New York, State University of New York Press,
1991
85
Fisher E, Auslander W, Sussman L, Owens N, Jackson-Thompson J: Community organization and health promotion in minority neighborhoods.
Ethn Dis
2
:
252
–272,
1992
86
Stolley MR, Fitzgibbon ML: Effects of an obesity prevention program on the eating behavior of African American mothers and daughters.
Health Educ Behav
24
:
152
–164,
1997
87
Campbell MK, Demark-Wahnefried W, Symons M, Kalsbeek WD, Dodds J, Cowan A, Jackson B, Motsinger B, Hoben K, Lashley J, Demissie S, McClelland JW: Fruit and vegetable consumption and prevention of cancer: the Black Churches United for Better Health project.
Am J Public Health
89
:
1390
–1396,
1999
88
Simmons D, Fleming C, Voyle J, Fou F, Feo S, Gatland B: A pilot urban church-based programme to reduce risk factors for diabetes among Western Samoans in New Zealand.
Diabet Med
15
:
136
–142,
1998

Address correspondence and reprint requests to Edwin B. Fisher, PhD, Division of Health Behavior Research, Washington University, 4444 Forest Park Ave., St. Louis, MO 63108. E-mail: [email protected].

Received for publication 29 June 2001 and accepted in revised form 6 December 2001.

A table elsewhere in this issue shows conventional and Système International (SI) units and conversion factors for many substances.