OBJECTIVE

Type 2 diabetes is known to be associated with decrements in memory and executive functions and information-processing speed. It is less clear, however, at which stage of diabetes these cognitive decrements develop and how they progress over time. In this study, we investigated cognitive functioning of patients with recent screen-detected type 2 diabetes, thus providing insight into the nature and severity of cognitive decrements in the early stage of the disease. Possible risk factors were also addressed.

RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS

Included in this study were 183 diabetic patients from a previously established study cohort and 69 control subjects. A full neuropsychological assessment, addressing six cognitive domains, was made for each participant. Raw test scores were standardized into z scores per domain and compared between the groups. Possible risk factors for cognitive decrements were examined with multivariate linear regression.

RESULTS

Relative to scores for the control group, mean z scores were between 0.01 and 0.2 lower in the diabetic group across all domains, but after adjustment for differences in IQ between patients and control subjects, only memory performance was significantly reduced (mean difference −0.15 [95% CI −0.28 to −0.03]). A history of macrovascular disease and current smoking were significant determinants of slower information-processing speed in patients with diabetes.

CONCLUSIONS

This study shows that modest cognitive decrements are already present at the early stage of type 2 diabetes. A history of macrovascular disease and smoking are significant risk factors for some early decrements.

Type 2 diabetes is associated with accelerated cognitive decline (1) and an increased risk of dementia (2,3), particularly in older individuals. Previous studies have shown decrements in memory function, executive function, and information-processing speed (4,5). These decrements in cognitive functioning are associated with modest brain atrophy and vascular lesions on brain magnetic resonance imaging (6). Diabetes-related factors, such as insulin resistance, chronic hyperglycemia, hypertension, and lipid disorders probably are relevant determinants (7,8).

It is unclear in which stage of diabetes the cognitive decrements become manifest and how they progress over time. Most studies have focused on patients with a known history of diabetes of several years (9). However, type 2 diabetes typically develops insidiously and may often be undiagnosed in the early stages. Therefore, cognitive decrements may start to develop years before the actual diagnosis, even in the pre-diabetes stages. Detailed neuropsychological data on the early stage of type 2 diabetes are not yet available. Moreover, possible risk factors for early cognitive decrements are not completely known.

In this study we assessed cognition in the early stage of diabetes by means of a detailed neuropsychological assessment (NPA) in a substantial population of patients with recent screen-detected diabetes. Possible risk factors were also addressed.

The Anglo-Danish-Dutch Study of Intensive Treatment in People with Screen Detected Diabetes in Primary Care (ADDITION) study is a multinational randomized trial involving 3,057 screen-detected type 2 diabetic patients that compares the effectiveness of an intensified multifactoral treatment with usual care on 5-year cardiovascular morbidity and mortality rates in a primary care setting (10). In the Netherlands, 56,987 individuals without known diabetes were offered a questionnaire, and those with a score above threshold underwent further glucose testing. Eventually, 586 participants had a diagnosis of type 2 diabetes according to the World Health Organization 1999 criteria (11), and 498 individuals were included in the study. Inclusion started in 2002 and ended in 2004.

In the ADDITION study, usual care is performed according to the different national guidelines from the three countries (in the Netherlands, the guidelines are from the Dutch College of General Practitioners [(12)]). The intensified multifactorial treatment consists of lifestyle advice regarding diet, physical activity, and smoking; protocol-driven strict regulation of blood glucose (A1C ≤6.5–7.0%), blood lipids (cholesterol <3.5 mmol/l), and blood pressure (<130/80 mmHg); and in those with blood pressure >120/80 mmHg prescription of acetylsalicylic acid and an ACE inhibitor. The primary outcome measure of the study is the combination of cardiovascular morbidity and mortality, all revascularizations, or nontraumatic amputations, whichever came first.

Inclusion in the cognition part of the ADDITION study

Cognition was assessed in an add-on project to the main ADDITION study in the Netherlands. Patients were invited to participate by an information letter from the study group and their family physician. Control subjects were peers of the patients, and both groups were matched for age, sex, and level of education. All patients and control subjects gave informed consent. Time between initial screening and inclusion in the Cognition part of the ADDITION study was 3–4 years.

Inclusion criteria

Inclusion and exclusion criteria for the present study were identical to those of the ADDITION study. All participants were aged 50–70 years at time of screening (2002–2004). Within 6 weeks after diagnosis of type 2 diabetes, treatment started. Randomization was performed at the practice level, so participants were treated according to the group (intensified treatment/usual care) their family physician had been randomly assigned to.

NPA was performed 3.6 ± 0.56 (mean ± SD) years after the screening date. Patients were excluded from the ADDITION trial if they were known to have a history of alcohol or drug abuse, psychosis, personality disorder, dementia, or emotional, psychological, or neurological disorder, unrelated to diabetes, that was likely to invalidate informed consent or limit their ability to comply with the protocol requirements. For both the ADDITION trial and the Cognition part, individuals with a previous noninvalidating stroke could participate. At the time of screening, participants with or treated for malignant disease or other disease that limited life expectance to <5 years were excluded. Control subjects had a fasting blood glucose <7.0 mmol/l, according to American Diabetes Association criteria (13).

NPA

The NPA was performed with a previously established test battery consisting of 12 verbal and nonverbal tasks addressing six cognitive domains (abstract reasoning, memory function, information-processing speed, attention and executive function, and visuoconstruction) as described previously (14). For the present study, the domain language comprehension was added and assessed with the Token Test (short form) (15). The domain memory was divided into four subdomains: working memory, immediate memory and learning rate, forgetting rate, and incidental memory (the amount of information that can be memorized if one was not explicitly asked to remember something) (14). IQ was measured by the Dutch version of the National Adult Reading Test (NART) (16). This test is constructed to estimate premorbid levels of intelligence and is relatively independent of brain damage acquired after adulthood (16).

A depression scale (Community Mental Health Assessment) (17) was used to assess the potential effect of mood disturbances on cognition. Scores ≥16 were labeled as depressive symptoms.

Physical examination and administration of the neuropsychological tests were performed at the patients' homes. The tests were administrated in a fixed order, and the entire battery took about 90 min to complete.

Participant characteristics and risk factor assessment

Demographic variables and possible risk factors were recorded in a standardized interview. Educational level was recorded using seven categories (1, <6 years of education; 2, 6 years; 3, 8 years; 4, 9 years; 5, 10–11 years; 6, 12–18 years; and 7, >18 years of education). Height and weight were measured, and BMI was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters. Smoking was classified as current, past, or never. Alcohol consumption was recorded using six categories (0, no alcohol at all; 1, up to 3 units/week; 2, 4–10 units/week; 3, 11–20 units/week; 4, 21–30 units/week; and 5, >30 units/week). Participants in category 5 were excluded. A1C (percent) and cholesterol level (millimoles per liter) were measured in the week of the NPA and analyzed at the regional hospital. Systolic and diastolic blood pressures (millimeters of mercury) were measured at the beginning and the end of the neuropsychological assessment; measurements were averaged. Hypertension was defined as a mean systolic blood pressure >160 mmHg, a mean diastolic blood pressure >95 mmHg, or use of blood pressure–lowering medication. These relatively high cutoff values were used because otherwise >90% of the patients would be classified as being hypertensive, which would hamper the assessment of the role of this risk factor in the regression analyses. Macrovascular disease was defined as history of myocardial infarction, stroke, or surgery or endovascular treatment for carotid, coronary, or peripheral arterial disease.

Analysis

The differences between patients and control subjects were examined with Student's t tests for means, Mann-Whitney U tests for nonparametric data, and χ2 tests for proportions. To analyze the difference in cognitive functioning between diabetic patients and control subjects, raw test scores for the NPA of both groups were standardized into z scores per domain. Mean z scores of the six cognitive domains were compared between the groups with univariate ANOVAs. Estimated mean differences between group differences were calculated and are presented with 95% CIs. Because the estimated premorbid IQ was significantly different between diabetic patients and control subjects, the NART-IQ was used as a covariate. To further assess the potential confounding effect of the NART-IQ imbalance, we performed a secondary analysis including all the control subjects (n = 69) and an exact age-, sex-, and NART-IQ–frequency matched selection of the patients (n = 143).

The relation between metabolic and vascular risk factors and cognition within the type 2 diabetic patients was assessed with linear regression analyses (adjusted for sex, age, and NART-IQ). To limit the number of analyses, only the domains of information-processing speed and memory were entered in these regression analyses, because these domains are known to be particularly sensitive to the effects of type 2 diabetes (9).

Participant Characteristics

A total of 183 patients with diabetes and 69 control subjects were included in the Cognition part of the ADDITION study. Patients and control subjects were balanced on sex, age, and educational level (Table 1). However, control subjects had a significantly higher estimated premorbid IQ.

Table 1

Participant characteristics

Patients with type 2 diabetesControl subjects
n 183 69 
Sex (% males) 61.2 47.8 
Mean age (years) 63.0 ± 5.4 62.7 ± 6.4 
Education level (1–7) 4 (4–5) 5 (4–6) 
Estimated premorbid IQ 96.7 ± 19.6 103.8 ± 16.3* 
BMI (kg/m230.4 ± 5.3 27.4 ± 4.2* 
Current smoking (%) 21.2 11.8 
Alcohol (0–5) 1 (0–2) 2 (1–3) 
Depressive symptoms (%) 9.8 5.8 
A1C (%) 6.2 ± 0.5 5.5 ± 0.3* 
Cholesterol (mmol/l) 4.1 ± 1.0 5.7 ± 1.0* 
Ùse lipid-lowering medication (%) 78.7 15.9* 
Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) 143 ± 20 140 ± 21 
Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) 82 ± 10 81 ± 12 
Hypertension (%) 85.2 36.2* 
Use antihypertensive drugs (%) 81.4 23.2* 
Macrovascular disease (%) 14.8 43* 
Patients with type 2 diabetesControl subjects
n 183 69 
Sex (% males) 61.2 47.8 
Mean age (years) 63.0 ± 5.4 62.7 ± 6.4 
Education level (1–7) 4 (4–5) 5 (4–6) 
Estimated premorbid IQ 96.7 ± 19.6 103.8 ± 16.3* 
BMI (kg/m230.4 ± 5.3 27.4 ± 4.2* 
Current smoking (%) 21.2 11.8 
Alcohol (0–5) 1 (0–2) 2 (1–3) 
Depressive symptoms (%) 9.8 5.8 
A1C (%) 6.2 ± 0.5 5.5 ± 0.3* 
Cholesterol (mmol/l) 4.1 ± 1.0 5.7 ± 1.0* 
Ùse lipid-lowering medication (%) 78.7 15.9* 
Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) 143 ± 20 140 ± 21 
Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) 82 ± 10 81 ± 12 
Hypertension (%) 85.2 36.2* 
Use antihypertensive drugs (%) 81.4 23.2* 
Macrovascular disease (%) 14.8 43* 

Data are means ± SD, proportion (in percent), and median (interquartile range) unless indicated otherwise.

*P < 0.01;

P < 0.05.

There were differences in the metabolic and vascular profiles between patients and control subjects (Table 1). Control subjects had a significantly lower BMI, and they consumed significantly more units of alcohol per week than the patient group.

Vascular risk factors in patients in both treatment groups were well controlled. Those who received multifactoral treatment had a slightly lower A1C level (mean ± SD difference –0.23 ± 0.07%), cholesterol (mean difference –0.53 ± 0.14 mmol/l), and mean arterial pressure (difference –3.05 ± 1.78 mmHg) than those who received usual care.

Cognitive functioning

The diabetic group performed significantly worse on memory functions, information-processing speed, attention, and executive functions and language comprehension in the unadjusted analyses, but the mean differences between the groups were small (–0.21 to –0.35) (Table 2). After adjustment for NART-IQ, only memory functions differed significantly between the groups (–0.15). The memory subdomains “immediate memory and learning rate” and “incidental memory” differed significantly between the groups after adjustment for NART-IQ. The results of the secondary analyses, in a selected subpopulation with exact matching for age, sex, and NART-IQ, showed an identical cognitive profile with similar effect sizes (results not shown). There were no significant differences in cognitive functioning between the patients who received multifactor treatment compared with patients who received usual care (results not shown).

Table 2

Estimated mean differences (95% CI)

UnadjustedAdjusted for NART-IQ
Abstract reasoning −0.20 (−0.48 to 0.08) −0.01 (−0.26 to 0.23) 
Memory −0.21 (−0.32 to −0.08) −0.15 (−0.28 to −0.03) 
    Working memory −0.20 (−0.42 to 0.01) −0.07 (−0.27 to 0.13) 
    Immediate memory and learning rate −0.24 (−0.41 to −0.06)* −0.18 (−0.35 to −0.003) 
    Forgetting rate 0.04 (−0.18 to 0.26) 0.04 (−0.18 to 0.26) 
    Incidental memory −0.49 (−0.73 to −0.17)* −0.42 (−0.71 to −0.14)* 
Information processing speed −0.26 (−0.48 to −0.03) −0.13 (−0.33 to 0.08) 
Attention and executive functions −0.23 (−0.42 to −0.04) −0.12 (−0.29 to 0.05) 
Visuoconstruction −0.23 (−0.52 to 0.05) −0.10 (−0.37 to 0.17) 
Language comprehension −0.35 (−0.65 to −0.04) −0.19 (−0.49 to 0.11) 
UnadjustedAdjusted for NART-IQ
Abstract reasoning −0.20 (−0.48 to 0.08) −0.01 (−0.26 to 0.23) 
Memory −0.21 (−0.32 to −0.08) −0.15 (−0.28 to −0.03) 
    Working memory −0.20 (−0.42 to 0.01) −0.07 (−0.27 to 0.13) 
    Immediate memory and learning rate −0.24 (−0.41 to −0.06)* −0.18 (−0.35 to −0.003) 
    Forgetting rate 0.04 (−0.18 to 0.26) 0.04 (−0.18 to 0.26) 
    Incidental memory −0.49 (−0.73 to −0.17)* −0.42 (−0.71 to −0.14)* 
Information processing speed −0.26 (−0.48 to −0.03) −0.13 (−0.33 to 0.08) 
Attention and executive functions −0.23 (−0.42 to −0.04) −0.12 (−0.29 to 0.05) 
Visuoconstruction −0.23 (−0.52 to 0.05) −0.10 (−0.37 to 0.17) 
Language comprehension −0.35 (−0.65 to −0.04) −0.19 (−0.49 to 0.11) 

*P < 0.01;

P < 0.05.

Possible risk factors

Age was inversely related with performance on tasks for memory and information-processing speed in diabetic patients (Table 3). Neither sex nor HbAl levels, blood pressure, cholesterol levels, or BMI was significantly related to cognitive performance. A history of macrovascular disease, however, was associated with reduced information-processing speed. Current smoking also had a significant effect on the reduced information-processing speed. Depressive symptoms were not significantly related to memory functions or information-processing speed. In control subjects, only age was inversely related with performances on memory and information-processing tasks (not shown in table).

Table 3

Determinants of performance on memory and information processing speed, adjusted for sex, age, and NART-IQ (regression analyses)

Patients with type 2 diabetes
Memory
Information processing speed
BβBβ
Age −0.02 (−0.04 to −0.01)* −0.26 −0.07 (−0.08 to −0.05)* −0.44 
Sex −0.03 (−0.16 to 0.10) −0.03 (−0.27 to 0.12) −0.04 
BMI 0.003 (−0.01 to 0.02) 0.03 −0.02 (−0.04 to 0.001) −0.11 
Current smoking −0.04 (−0.20 to 0.12) −0.03 −0.26 (−0.50 to −0.03) −0.13 
A1C 0.004 (−0.13 to 0.14) 0.004 0.004 (−0.20 to 0.20) 0.002 
Cholesterol 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.09) 0.04 −0.07 (−0.17 to 0.03) −0.08 
Hypertension 0.06 (−0.13 to 0.24) 0.04 −0.17 (−0.47 to 0.10) −0.08 
Systolic blood pressure (per 10 mmHg) 0.001 (−0.03 to 0.04) 0.01 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.04 
Diastolic blood pressure (per 10 mmHg) −0.04 (−0.10 to 0.03) −0.08 −0.04 (−0.13 to 0.05) −0.05 
Macrovascular disease −0.13 (−0.28 to 0.02) −0.12 −0.30 (−0.51 to −0.09)* −0.17 
Depressive symptoms −0.08 (−0.31 to 0.14) −0.05 0.01 (−0.34 to 0.35) 0.002 
Patients with type 2 diabetes
Memory
Information processing speed
BβBβ
Age −0.02 (−0.04 to −0.01)* −0.26 −0.07 (−0.08 to −0.05)* −0.44 
Sex −0.03 (−0.16 to 0.10) −0.03 (−0.27 to 0.12) −0.04 
BMI 0.003 (−0.01 to 0.02) 0.03 −0.02 (−0.04 to 0.001) −0.11 
Current smoking −0.04 (−0.20 to 0.12) −0.03 −0.26 (−0.50 to −0.03) −0.13 
A1C 0.004 (−0.13 to 0.14) 0.004 0.004 (−0.20 to 0.20) 0.002 
Cholesterol 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.09) 0.04 −0.07 (−0.17 to 0.03) −0.08 
Hypertension 0.06 (−0.13 to 0.24) 0.04 −0.17 (−0.47 to 0.10) −0.08 
Systolic blood pressure (per 10 mmHg) 0.001 (−0.03 to 0.04) 0.01 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.04 
Diastolic blood pressure (per 10 mmHg) −0.04 (−0.10 to 0.03) −0.08 −0.04 (−0.13 to 0.05) −0.05 
Macrovascular disease −0.13 (−0.28 to 0.02) −0.12 −0.30 (−0.51 to −0.09)* −0.17 
Depressive symptoms −0.08 (−0.31 to 0.14) −0.05 0.01 (−0.34 to 0.35) 0.002 

Data are regression coefficient B (95% CI) and standardized β.

*P < 0.01;

P < 0.05.

This study shows that patients with recent screen-detected type 2 diabetes performed significantly worse on memory functions, in particular, the immediate and the incidental memory, compared with control subjects. A history of macrovascular diseases and current smoking were the strongest determinants of a lower information-processing speed in the diabetic group.

The effect sizes for the difference in cognition between the diabetic and control groups found in this study are small compared with those in other studies (9), possibly reflecting the relatively short duration of diabetes in our population. Indeed, in a previous study with the same NPA battery, we found effect sizes of 0.3–0.4 among patients with a mean diabetes duration of 8 years (8). Another study using the same assessment battery in patients with a diabetes duration of 5–9 years showed effect sizes of 0.2–0.3 (18). Diabetes duration thus seems to be linked to the effect sizes of the studies: the longer the known diabetes duration, the bigger the effect size. In the present study we observed a small difference in language comprehension between patients and control subjects that was not significant after adjustment for NART-IQ. The meaning of this finding is not clear. The domain language comprehension is seldom addressed in studies on cognition in patients with type 2 diabetes. Moderate correlations between the token test and measures of short-term memory have been reported in a previous study on nondiabetic subjects (19); however, our results do not indicate that our patients performed worse on measures of short-term memory (working memory). The observed small effect on this test is well outside the range of what would be considered as abnormal performance and is therefore unlikely to confound performance on the other cognitive tests.

Further research is necessary to see how the cognitive decrement in our patients will develop over time and whether they also will develop problems in executive functions and information-processing speed as described in other studies. The patients included in this study will be followed over time, and a second NPA will be performed in a few years.

The relation between macrovascular diseases, smoking, and cognition has also been found in previous studies of patients with diabetes (8,20). Traditionally, hypertension is also thought to mediate the association between diabetes and cognitive dysfunction (4,21), but results of previous, mostly cross-sectional, studies do not consistently show this relation, in line with our findings. Also in nondiabetic subjects, the association between hypertension and cognitive functioning varies with age and time of exposure and is most evident when blood pressure is assessed in midlife and cognition in late life (22). Therefore, the association may be less evident in a cross-sectional study in a relatively older population, such as ours.

Regarding glycemic control, the literature mostly shows a negative relation between A1C (chronic exposure to hyperglycemia) and cognition in type 1 (23) and type 2 diabetes (24,25). We could not confirm this relationship. It is possible that we did not find this relationship because of the relatively strict metabolic control in our patients, but it is also possible that the negative effect of A1C on cognition becomes more evident after longer diabetes duration.

A strength of our study is the measurement of cognitive functions in the early stage of the disease. Previous studies focused mainly on patients with a longer diabetes duration. This study gives more information on early cognitive decrements and shows, in combination with other studies that used the same NPA in patients with a longer duration of diabetes, that the decrements seem to be progressive over time.

A limitation of our study is the difference in IQ scores of patients and control subjects. Control subjects had significantly higher IQ scores compared with those for the diabetic patients. We therefore had to adjust the analyses for NART-IQ. In a secondary analysis with exact matching for age, sex, and NART-IQ, patients still performed poorer on memory functions than control subjects. Besides a difference in IQ scores, there also was a nonsignificant higher proportion of men in the patient group, but sex was unrelated to performances in any cognitive domain (Table 3).

Another limitation is the time between screening and the NPA, which is between 3 and 4 years. Although this period is relatively short, we cannot say anything about the cognitive functioning in the first stage of type 2 diabetes. On the other hand, because of the screening procedure, the diabetic patients in our study are likely to have had their diabetes diagnosed some years earlier; thus, they may be in the same period of their disease as patients in usual care with a recent diagnosis of type 2 diabetes.

Because of the delay between screening and the NPA, half of the diabetic patients had received multifactoral intensified treatment for a period of 3–4 years. Although levels of A1C, cholesterol, and blood pressure were indeed better in the intensively treated group, both groups showed good control for these risk factors, and no effect of treatment allocation on cognition was observed in the present interim analysis. It is possible that a longer treatment duration or contrast in risk factor levels between the groups is required to observe effects on cognition. This possibility will be addressed in the follow-up study, once the treatment period has been completed.

In summary, cognitive decrements can be found in the early stages of type 2 diabetes. This finding may have implications for diabetes education and self-management behavior in diabetic patients. Diabetes educators should at least take into account the immediate memory and learning rate and the incidental memory of patients with a recent diagnosis of diabetes. If one wishes to prevent diabetes-associated cognitive decrements, interventions may need to be initiated at a very early stage. Offering a smoking cessation consultation would be the best option in those patients who are smokers. Whether other therapies might be beneficial to decrease the risk on cognitive impairment remains uncertain.

The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

The ADDITION study in the Netherlands was funded by grants from Novo Nordisk Netherlands, GlaxoSmithKline Netherlands, and Merck Netherlands. No other potential conflicts of interest relevant to this article were reported.

1.
Cukierman
T
,
Gerstein
HC
,
Williamson
JD
.
Cognitive decline and dementia in diabetes—systematic overview of prospective observational studies
.
Diabetologia
2005
; 
48
:
2460
2469
2.
Arvanitakis
Z
,
Wilson
RS
,
Bienias
JL
,
Evans
DA
,
Bennett
DA
.
Diabetes mellitus and risk of Alzheimer disease and decline in cognitive function
.
Arch Neurol
2004
; 
61
:
661
666
3.
Ott
A
,
Stolk
RP
,
van Harskamp
F
,
Pols
HAP
,
Hofman
A
,
Breteler
MMB
.
Diabetes mellitus and the risk of dementia
.
Neurology
1999
; 
53
:
1937
1942
4.
Stewart
R
,
Liolitsa
D
.
Type 2 diabetes mellitus, cognitive impairment and dementia
.
Diabet Med
1999
; 
16
:
93
112
5.
van den Berg
E
,
Kessels
RPC
,
Kappelle
LJ
,
de Haan
EHF
,
Biessels
GJ
.
Type 2 diabetes, cognitive function and dementia: vascular and metabolic determinants
.
Drugs Today
2006
; 
42
:
741
754
6.
Manschot
SM
,
Brands
AMA
,
van der Grond
J
,
Kessels
RPC
,
Algra
A
,
Kappelle
LJ
,
Biessels
GJ
.
Brain magnetic resonance imaging correlates of impaired cognition in patients with type 2 diabetes
.
Diabetes
2006
; 
55
:
1106
1113
7.
Biessels
GJ
,
Staekenburg
S
,
Brunner
E
,
Brayne
C
,
Scheltens
P
.
Risk of dementia in diabetes mellitus: a systematic review
.
Lancet Neurol
2006
; 
5
:
64
74
8.
Manschot
SM
,
Biessels
GJ
,
de Valk
H
,
Algra
A
,
Rutten
GEHM
,
van der Grond
J
,
Kappelle
LJ
.
Metabolic and vascular determinants of impaired cognitive performance and abnormalities on brain magnetic resonance imaging in patients with type 2 diabetes
.
Diabetologia
2007
; 
50
:
2388
2397
9.
Awad
N
,
Gagnon
M
,
Messier
C
.
The relationship between impaired glucose tolerance, type 2 diabetes, and cognitive function
.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
2004
; 
26
:
1044
1080
10.
Sandbaek
A
,
Griffin
SJ
,
Rutten
G
,
Davies
M
,
Stolk
R
,
Khunti
K
,
Borch-Johnsen
K
,
Wareham
NJ
,
Lauritzen
T
.
Stepwise screening for diabetes identifies people with high but modifiable coronary heart disease risk: the ADDITION study
.
Diabetologia
2008
; 
51
:
1127
1134
11.
Alberti
KGMM
,
Zimmet
PZ
.
Definition, diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus and its complications. Part 1: diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus. Provisional report of a WHO consultation
.
Diabet Med
1998
; 
15
:
539
553
12.
Rutten
GEHM
,
De Grauw
WJC
,
Nijpels
G
,
Goudswaard
AN
,
Uitdewaal
PJM
,
Van der Does
FEE
,
Heine
RJ
,
Van Ballegooie
E
,
Verduijn
MM
,
Bouma
M
.
NHG-standard diabetes mellitus type 2
.
Huisarts Wetenschap
2006
; 
49
:
137
152
13.
American Diabetes Association
.
Standards of medical care for patients with diabetes mellitus
.
Diabetes Care
25
:
213
229
14.
Brands
AMA
,
Kessels
RPC
,
Hoogma
RPLM
,
Henselmans
JML
,
van der Beek Boter
JW
,
Kappelle
LJ
,
de Haan
EHF
,
Biessels
GJ
.
Cognitive performance, psychological well-being, and brain magnetic resonance imaging in older patients with type 1 diabetes
.
Diabetes
2006
; 
55
:
1800
1806
15.
Graetz
P
,
de Bleser
R
,
Willmes
K
.
Akense Afasie Test.
Lisse, the Netherlands
,
Swets & Zeitlinger
,
1992
16.
Schmand
B
,
Lindeboom
J
,
van Harskamp
F
.
Nederlandse Leestest voor Volwassenen [Dutch Adult Reading Test].
Lisse, the Netherlands
,
Swets & Zeitlinger
,
1992
17.
Bouma
J
,
Ranchor
AV
,
Sanderman
R
,
van Sonderen
E
.
Het meten van symptomen van depressie met de CES-D: een handleiding.
Groningen, the Netherlands
,
Noordelijk Centrum voor Gezondheidsvraagstukken
,
1995
18.
van den Berg
E
,
Dekker
J
,
Nijpels
G
,
Kessels
RPC
,
Kappelle
LJ
,
de Haan
EHF
,
Heine
RJ
,
Stehouwer
CDA
,
Biessels
GJ
.
Cognitive functioning in elderly persons with type 2 diabetes and metabolic syndrome: the Hoorn study
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
2008
; 
26
:
261
269
19.
Lesser
R
.
Verbal and non-verbal memory components in the Token Test
.
Neuropsychologia
1976
; 
14
:
79
85
20.
Arvanitakis
Z
,
Wilson
RS
,
Li
Y
,
Aggerwal
NT
,
Bennett
DA
.
Diabetes and function in different cognitive systems in older individuals without dementia
.
Diabetes Care
2006
; 
29
:
560
565
21.
Hassing
LB
,
Hofer
SM
,
Nilsson
SE
,
Berg
S
,
Pedersen
NL
,
McClearn
G
,
Johansson
B
.
Comorbid type 2 diabetes mellitus and hypertension exacerbates cognitive decline: evidence from a longitudinal study
.
Age Ageing
2004
; 
33
:
355
361
22.
Kloppenborg
RP
,
van den Berg
E
,
Kappelle
LJ
,
Biessels
GJ
.
Diabetes and other vascular factors for dementia: which factor matters most? A systematic review
.
Eur J Pharmacol
2008
; 
585
:
97
108
23.
Brands
AMA
,
Biessels
GJ
,
de Haan
EHF
,
Kappelle
LJ
,
Kessels
RPC
.
The effects of type 1 diabetes on cognitive performance
.
Diabetes Care
2005
; 
29
:
726
735
24.
van Harten
B
,
Oosterman
J
,
Muslimovic
D
,
van Loon
BJP
,
Scheltens
P
,
Weinstein
HC
.
Cognitive impairment and MRI correlates in the elderly patients with type 2 diabetes
.
Age Ageing
2007
; 
36
:
164
170
25.
Munshi
M
,
Grande
L
,
Hayes
M
,
Ayres
D
,
Suhl
E
,
Capelson
R
,
Lin
S
,
Milberg
W
,
Weinger
K
.
Cognitive dysfunction is associated with poor diabetes control in older adults
.
Diabetes Care
2006
; 
29
:
1794
1799
Readers may use this article as long as the work is properly cited, the use is educational and not for profit, and the work is not altered. See http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/ for details.