Day-to-day functioning is a fundamental consideration when evaluating the health and quality of life of people with type 1 diabetes (1). However, despite many decades of research on the immediate and long-term impacts of hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia, we know relatively little about the functional impact of glucose variability and glycemic excursions over hours and days in real-world environments. New ambulatory technologies for monitoring glucose, activity, and cognitive function allow researchers to fill this gap, which may ultimately lead to better interventions and patient outcomes.

We know that adults with type 1 diabetes have (on average) poorer cognitive performance than those without diabetes (24). It is also well established that poorer cognitive performance is linked to worse functional outcomes and more difficulties with diabetes self-management (2,5,6). Factors associated with cognitive impairment in type 1 diabetes include age of onset, HbA1c, severe hypoglycemic events, and diabetes complications (3,79). Hypoglycemic and hyperglycemic clamp studies have demonstrated that acute changes in blood glucose can result in reversible cognitive dysfunction (1015). However, these general findings obscure a tremendous amount of heterogeneity among individuals and in patient outcomes that leave many questions unanswered (16). Previous research on the cognitive and psychological impacts of hypoglycemia, for example, demonstrates that such impacts can vary between individuals (17) and within individuals over time (18). The factors that influence whether a glycemic excursion impacts short-term cognitive status in a given person on a given day remain largely unknown. Are there glycemic, psychological, and diabetes-related factors that make an individual more or less likely to experience short-term cognitive impairments in everyday environments? As research shifts toward precision medicine approaches, there is increasing acknowledgment that group-level data can obscure meaningful individual differences and that diagnosis and treatment can benefit from approaches that are ecologically sensitive (19).

Using continuous glucose monitoring, actigraphy, and mobile technology, the study by Pyatak et al. (1) begins to fill this gap through a comprehensive investigation of the everyday associations between glucose and functioning and the individual-level factors that may moderate this association. Specifically, Pyatak et al. examined associations between overnight glucose metrics (coefficient of variation, time spent <70 mg/dL, and time spent >250 mg/dL) and next-day functioning. Next-day functioning was estimated based on cognitive performance, self-reported activity, and accelerometer-based actigraphy over 14 days in a sample of individuals with type 1 diabetes. Data were collected using mobile cognitive tests, and surveys were administered every 3 h throughout the day. They found that greater glucose variability overnight (based on coefficient of variation) and percent time with glucose >250 mg/dL was associated with poorer next-day functioning. Higher nocturnal glucose variability was specifically associated with poorer sustained attention and lower engagement in demanding activities the following day. Overnight hyperglycemia was associated with more sedentary time the following day. Some of these effects were at least partially explained by the impact of glycemic fluctuations on sleep. For example, higher overnight glucose variability led to more fragmented sleep, which explained part of the association between glucose variability and lower sustained attention the next day. This suggests that interventions that improve sleep can reduce the impact of glucose variability on next-day cognitive performance.

Importantly, Pyatak et al. (1) also found that the association between overnight hypoglycemia and next-day attention was linked to patient-reported outcomes like global illness intrusiveness and diabetes-related quality of life. This may represent a specific mechanistic determinant of quality of life among people with type 1 diabetes that could be addressed by behavioral interventions.

There are many strengths to this study, such as the sample size and the comprehensive approach to measuring next-day function through both subjective (self-reported activities) and objective (cognitive tests and actigraphy) measures. The sample was also racially and ethnically diverse, which is an important characteristic given the large race-related disparities in type 1 diabetes health outcomes (20).

One potential weakness of the study is that the effect sizes reported were quite small. However, accumulated day by day over a lifetime, these effects are likely important for those living with type 1 diabetes (i.e., “death by a thousand paper cuts”), and for interventions that aim to improve daily functioning. Indeed, the science of everyday life is typically a science of small effects. For instance, the impact of sleep and stress on next-day functioning (21,22) and the impact of circadian rhythms on cognitive performance tend to be small (23) but meaningful enough that even healthy individuals notice the impact. For individuals, knowing how, when, and in what way their diabetes impacts their everyday functioning (and subjective experience of their everyday functioning) is a critical part of patient-centered science. The work by Pyatak et al. demonstrates that, as a field, our science and technology tools are finally poised to meet this need.

The findings from this study, while important in their own right, also provide a framework that can be used to answer many other questions about the underlying factors that predict daily functioning in individuals with type 1 diabetes. This knowledge is key to developing interventions that can make meaningful change in group outcomes that also matter to individuals. Ultimately, work like this will lead to tools that let patients self-monitor and identify the factors that impact their own daily functioning using continuous glucose monitoring coupled with other mobile technology prediction tools. This personalized data could be used clinically or by the individual to improve day-to-day functioning (e.g., for academic and work performance). For example, even subtle and temporary cognitive impairments can lead to accidents as well as financial or self-management mistakes, which can have disastrous impacts on well-being, health, and independence. Finally, results like these will help generate hypotheses about the brain mechanisms that underlie everyday functional impairments and resiliency in individuals with type 1 diabetes. It is our hope that studies like this one will lead to rational interventions that can minimize the impact of glycemic excursions on cognitive and functional status, allowing patients to maximize their functioning and quality of life in everyday environments.

See accompanying article, p. 1345.

Funding. L.T.G. and N.S.C. receive funding from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, National Institutes of Health (R01 DK121240).

Duality of Interest. No potential conflicts of interest relevant to this article were reported.

1.
Pyatak
EA
,
Spruijt-Metz
D
,
Schneider
S
, et al
.
Impact of overnight glucose on next-day functioning in adults with type 1 diabetes: an exploratory intensive longitudinal study
.
Diabetes Care
2023
;
46
:
1345
1353
2.
Chaytor
NS
,
Barbosa-Leiker
C
,
Ryan
CM
,
Germine
LT
,
Hirsch
IB
,
Weinstock
RS
.
Clinically significant cognitive impairment in older adults with type 1 diabetes
.
J Diabetes Complications
2019
;
33
:
91
97
3.
Brands
AM
,
Biessels
GJ
,
de Haan
EH
,
Kappelle
LJ
,
Kessels
RP
.
The effects of type 1 diabetes on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis
.
Diabetes Care
2005
;
28
:
726
735
4.
Ohmann
S
,
Popow
C
,
Rami
B
, et al
.
Cognitive functions and glycemic control in children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes
.
Psychol Med
2010
;
40
:
95
103
5.
Feil
DG
,
Zhu
CW
,
Sultzer
DL
.
The relationship between cognitive impairment and diabetes self-management in a population-based community sample of older adults with type 2 diabetes
.
J Behav Med
2012
;
35
:
190
199
6.
Lippa
KD
,
Klein
HA
,
Shalin
VL
.
Everyday expertise: cognitive demands in diabetes selfmanagement
.
Hum Factors
2008
;
50
:
112
120
7.
Nunley
KA
,
Rosano
C
,
Ryan
CM
, et al
.
Clinically relevant cognitive impairment in middle-aged adults with childhood-onset type 1 diabetes
.
Diabetes Care
2015
;
38
:
1768
1776
8.
Tonoli
C
,
Heyman
E
,
Roelands
B
, et al
.
Type 1 diabetes-associated cognitive decline: a meta-analysis and update of the current literature
.
J Diabetes
2014
;
6
:
499
513
9.
Ryan
CM
,
Klein
BEK
,
Lee
KE
,
Cruickshanks
KJ
,
Klein
R
.
Associations between recent severe hypoglycemia, retinal vessel diameters, and cognition in adults with type 1 diabetes
.
J Diabetes Complications
2016
;
30
:
1513
1518
10.
Allen
KV
,
Pickering
MJ
,
Zammitt
NN
, et al
.
Effects of acute hypoglycemia on working memory and language processing in adults with and without type 1 diabetes
.
Diabetes Care
2015
;
38
:
1108
1115
11.
Ewing
FM
,
Deary
IJ
,
McCrimmon
RJ
,
Strachan
MW
,
Frier
BM
.
Effect of acute hypoglycemia on visual information processing in adults with type 1 diabetes mellitus
.
Physiol Behav
1998
;
64
:
653
660
12.
McAulay
V
,
Deary
IJ
,
Ferguson
SC
,
Frier
BM
.
Acute hypoglycemia in humans causes attentional dysfunction while nonverbal intelligence is preserved
.
Diabetes Care
2001
;
24
:
1745
1750
13.
Sommerfield
AJ
,
Deary
IJ
,
McAulay
V
,
Frier
BM
.
Moderate hypoglycemia impairs multiple memory functions in healthy adults
.
Neuropsychology
2003
;
17
:
125
132
14.
Sommerfield
AJ
,
Deary
IJ
,
Frier
BM
.
Acute hyperglycemia alters mood state and impairs cognitive performance in people with type 2 diabetes
.
Diabetes Care
2004
;
27
:
2335
2340
15.
Wright
RJ
,
Frier
BM
,
Deary
IJ
.
Effects of acute insulin-induced hypoglycemia on spatial abilities in adults with type 1 diabetes
.
Diabetes Care
2009
;
32
:
1503
1506
16.
Draelos
MT
,
Jacobson
AM
,
Weinger
K
, et al
.
Cognitive function in patients with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus during hyperglycemia and hypoglycemia
.
Am J Med
1995
;
98
:
135
144
17.
Gold
AE
,
MacLeod
KM
,
Deary
IJ
,
Frier
BM
.
Hypoglycemia-induced cognitive dysfunction in diabetes mellitus: effect of hypoglycemia unawareness
.
Physiol Behav
1995
;
58
:
501
511
18.
Fanelli
CG
,
Epifano
L
,
Rambotti
AM
, et al
.
Meticulous prevention of hypoglycemia normalizes the glycemic thresholds and magnitude of most of neuroendocrine responses to, symptoms of, and cognitive function during hypoglycemia in intensively treated patients with short-term IDDM
.
Diabetes
1993
;
42
:
1683
1689
19.
Lenze
EJ
,
Nicol
GE
,
Barbour
DL
, et al
.
Precision clinical trials: a framework for getting to precision medicine for neurobehavioural disorders
.
J Psychiatry Neurosci
2021
;
46
:
E97
E110
20.
Lipman
TH
,
Smith
JA
,
Patil
O
,
Willi
SM
,
Hawkes
CP
.
Racial disparities in treatment and outcomes of children with type 1 diabetes
.
Pediatr Diabetes
2021
;
22
:
241
248
21.
Lee
S
,
Buxton
OM
,
Andel
R
,
Almeida
DM
.
Bidirectional associations of sleep with cognitive interference in employees’ work days
.
Sleep Health
2019
;
5
:
298
308
22.
Hyun
J
,
Sliwinski
MJ
,
Smyth
JM
.
Waking up on the wrong side of the bed: the effects of stress anticipation on working memory in daily life
.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
2019
;
74
:
38
46
23.
Riley
E
,
Esterman
M
,
Fortenbaugh
FC
,
DeGutis
J
.
Time-of-day variation in sustained attentional control
.
Chronobiol Int
2017
;
34
:
993
1001
Readers may use this article as long as the work is properly cited, the use is educational and not for profit, and the work is not altered. More information is available at https://www.diabetesjournals.org/journals/pages/license.