OBJECTIVE

To investigate the relationship between fasting glucose levels, insulin resistance, and cognitive impairment in old age. Diabetes is associated with cognitive impairment in older people. However, the link between elevated fasting glucose levels and insulin resistance in nondiabetic individuals, and the risk of cognitive impairment is unclear.

RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS

We analyzed data from, in total, 8,447 participants in two independent prospective studies: the PROspective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk (PROSPER), 5,019 participants, aged 69–84 years, and the Rotterdam Study, 3,428 participants, aged 61–97 years. Fasting glucose levels were assessed at baseline in both studies; fasting insulin levels were assessed in the Rotterdam Study only. Cognitive function was assessed in both studies at baseline and during follow-up.

RESULTS

Subjects with diabetes had impaired cognitive function at baseline. In contrast, in people without a history of diabetes, there was no clear association between baseline fasting glucose levels and executive function and memory, nor was there a consistent relationship between elevated baseline fasting glucose levels and the rate of cognitive decline in either cohort. Insulin resistance (homeostasis model assessment index) was also unrelated to cognitive function and decline.

CONCLUSIONS

Elevated fasting glucose levels and insulin resistance are not associated with worse cognitive function in older people without a history of diabetes. These data suggest either that there is a threshold for effects of dysglycemia on cognitive function or that factors other than hyperglycemia contribute to cognitive impairment in individuals with frank diabetes.

Diabetes has been shown to be associated with an increased risk of dementia and impaired cognitive function (1). Suggested biological mechanisms that are involved in this relation are accelerated cerebrovascular disease (1), accumulation of advanced glycation end products (2), and reduced amyloid β-clearance through disturbing the role of the insulin-degrading enzyme (3). The accumulating evidence that diabetes is involved in a number of health problems, ranging from retinopathy and cardiovascular symptoms to neurological complications, has started a discussion about the necessity to identify those people who are at increased risk for diabetes (3,,6).

Classifying people based on levels of fasting glucose to indicate “impaired fasting glucose” has been suggested as a possible tool for risk assessment of the development of diabetes (5,6). However, the relationship between the preceding stage of diabetes, when impaired fasting glucose levels are present, and cognitive function has not been comprehensively elucidated. A number of studies have investigated the relationship between this “pre-diabetes” state and cognitive function but showed contradictory or inconclusive results (7,9), possibly due to relatively small sample sizes and limited numbers of participants with an impaired fasting glucose level. Alternatively, peripheral insulin resistance that could underlie the elevated fasting glucose levels in the pre-diabetes state may contribute to impaired cognitive function (10,11).

Therefore, in this study we investigated the association between fasting glucose levels and cognitive function and decline in a large sample of 8,447 participants for whom fasting glucose levels at baseline were available together with longitudinal data from a dedicated neuropsychological test battery. Additionally, we investigated the relationship between insulin resistance (using the homeostasis model assessment [HOMA] index) and cognitive function and decline in 3,342 participants.

Populations.

The PROspective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk (PROSPER) was a prospective multicenter randomized placebo-controlled trial to assess whether treatment with pravastatin diminishes the risk of major cardiovascular events in the elderly (12,13). Between December 1997 and May 1999, a total of 5,804 participants (aged 70–82 years) with preexisting vascular disease or increased risk of such disease due to a history of smoking, hypertension, or diabetes were recruited in Scotland, Ireland, and the Netherlands. The institutional ethics review boards of all centers approved the protocol, and all participants gave written informed consent. Participants with very severe cognitive impairment (Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE] score <24) were excluded for inclusion in the study.

The Rotterdam Study is a large prospective population-based cohort study that is conducted among all inhabitants aged ≥55 years of Ommoord, a district of Rotterdam, the Netherlands (14). The medical ethics committee of the Erasmus University of Rotterdam approved the study, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Of 10,275 eligible subjects, 7,983 individuals (78%) participated in the baseline examinations between 1990 and 1993 (mean age 71 ± 25 years, range 55–106 years). All participants were interviewed at home and visited the research center for further examinations.

Fasting glucose levels.

In PROSPER, fasting glucose levels were assessed at baseline in 5,599 of the 5,804 participants. Of the 5,599 participants, 580 did not have all cognitive function tests available at baseline. All of the resulting 5,019 participants had full data available for other cardiovascular risk factors including BMI, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, and HDL cholesterol levels at baseline. This resulted in a study sample of 5,019 participants for PROSPER.

In the Rotterdam Study, fasting glucose levels were assessed at the third survey in 3,795 participants. Of these participants, 3,664 were free of dementia, and 3,550 of those had data available for other cardiovascular risk factors including BMI, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, and HDL cholesterol levels. Of these 3,550 participants, 122 did not have all cognitive function tests available at the start of the third survey. This resulted in a study sample of 3,428 participants for the Rotterdam Study. Additionally, in 3,342 of these 3,428 participants, fasting insulin levels were assessed at the third survey.

In both the PROSPER and the Rotterdam sample, fasting glucose levels were additionally measured during follow-up. There were 4,690 of the 5,019 participants in the PROSPER sample and 2,364 of the 3,428 participants in the Rotterdam sample who underwent at least one additional measurement of fasting glucose level in addition to the initial examination. These data were used to study the variability of the fasting glucose levels over time and to assess the appropriateness of using a single baseline fasting glucose measurement to assess the relationship between fasting glucose and cognitive function and decline.

History of diabetes.

At baseline, history of diabetes was defined by self-reported history of diabetes (reporting the use of oral antidiabetes medication, the use of insulin, treatment by diet, or registration by a general practitioner as having diabetes) in both study samples.

Cognitive function.

Global cognitive function was measured with the MMSE (15) in both studies. In addition, a dedicated neuropsychological test battery was used to assess executive function and memory. Executive function was assessed with the Letter–Digit Substitution Task (LDST) (16) and the abbreviated Stroop test part 3 (17) in both studies, as well as with the Word Fluency Test (WFT) (18) in the Rotterdam Study only. Memory was assessed with the 12-Picture Learning Test (12-PLT) immediate and delayed recall (19), in PROSPER only.

Individual test scores were transformed into standardized z scores [z score = (individual score – mean population score)/SD population score]. A compound cognitive test score for global cognitive function was calculated by averaging the z scores of the MMSE, the LDST, and the abbreviated Stroop test part 3. A compound cognitive test score for the 12-PLT was calculated by averaging the z scores of the 12-PLT immediate and the 12-PLT delayed recall test.

In PROSPER, cognitive function was measured at six time points during the study: before randomization, at baseline, after 9, 18, and 30 months, and at the end of the study. The time point of the last measurement was different for the participants and ranged from 36 to 48 months after baseline. Therefore, we performed the analyses with their individual varying time point but report the results for the mean of these time points (at 42 months). The prerandomized measurement was discarded in the analyses to preclude possible learning effects. This resulted in a mean follow-up of 3.2 years. Change in cognitive function could be assessed in 4,767 participants for whom at least one follow-up examination of cognitive function was available after the initial measurement.

In the Rotterdam Study, cognitive function was assessed at the third survey (1997–1999) and additionally at the fourth survey (2002–2004). This resulted in a mean follow-up of 4.6 years. Of the 3,428 participants of the Rotterdam sample who were present at the third survey, 2,601 remained in the study until the end of follow-up (fourth survey) and were available for the assessment of change in cognitive function.

Additional assessments.

In both samples, level of education, BMI, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, HDL cholesterol level, and APOE ε4 carriership were assessed at baseline (PROSPER) or at the third survey (Rotterdam Study). Level of education was dichotomized into primary education or less (low) and more than primary education (high) (Rotterdam Study) and into age when leaving school <13 years (low) and age when leaving school ≥13 years (high) (PROSPER).

Statistical analyses.

The relationship between baseline (PROSPER) or third survey (Rotterdam Study) fasting glucose levels and cognitive function and decline was assessed by use of linear mixed models. Data from the PROSPER sample and the Rotterdam Study sample were merged into one large sample of 8,447 participants. All analyses were adjusted for age, sex, level of education, study (PROSPER or Rotterdam Study), BMI, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, HDL cholesterol level, APOE ε4 carriership, country, use of pravastatin, and, where appropriate, test version. Analyses were carried out using the SPSS statistical package (release 12.0.1; SPSS, Chicago, IL).

Data on fasting glucose and fasting insulin levels from the Rotterdam Study sample were used to calculate the degree of insulin resistance according to HOMA (20). The HOMA index is calculated by dividing the product of fasting levels of glucose and insulin by a constant and has been shown to correlate well (r = 0.82, P < 0.0001) with the euglycemic-hyperinsulinemic clamp method (21).

Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of the total sample and for participants with a history of diabetes at baseline (PROSPER) or at the third survey (Rotterdam Study). In PROSPER, the fasting glucose levels ± SD differed among the three countries from which the participants were enrolled: Scotland 5.62 ± 1.27, Ireland 5.09 ± 1.34), and the Netherlands 5.76 ± 1.64 mmol/l. This resulted in a lower mean fasting glucose level for the PROSPER study sample compared with that of the Rotterdam Study sample. Participants with a history of diabetes had a higher fasting glucose level, BMI, and systolic blood pressure and lower levels of HDL cholesterol compared with participants without a history of diabetes.

TABLE 1

Baseline characteristics

Total sampleHistory of diabetes
NoYes
PROSPER    
    n 5,019 4,476 543 
    Age (years) 75.3 ± 3.3 75.3 ± 3.4 75.2 ± 3.2 
    Female sex 2,630 (52) 2,387 (53) 243 (45) 
    Low level of education 122 (2) 99 (2) 23 (4) 
    MMSE score (points) 28.1 ± 1.5 28.1 ± 1.5 28.0 ± 1.6 
    Fasting glucose levels (mmol/l) 5.45 ± 1.41 5.14 ± 0.78 8.02 ± 2.43 
    BMI (kg/m226.9 ± 4.2 26.7 ± 4.1 28.0 ± 4.3 
    Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) 155 ± 22 154 ± 22 158 ± 22 
    Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) 84 ± 11 84 ± 11 84 ± 11 
    HDL cholesterol (mmol/l) 1.28 ± 0.35 1.29 ± 0.35 1.17 ± 0.31 
    APOE ε4 carriership 1,192 (24) 1,091 (24) 101 (19) 
Rotterdam Study    
    n 3,428 3,116 312 
    Age (years) 71.8 ± 6.6 71.6 ± 6.5 73.8 ± 6.8 
    Female sex 1,975 (58) 1,817 (58) 158 (51) 
    Low level of education 515 (15) 472 (15) 43 (14) 
    MMSE score (points) 27.8 ± 1.8 27.8 ± 1.8 27.6 ± 1.9 
    Fasting glucose levels (mmol/l) 5.88 ± 1.35 5.65 ± 0.93 8.12 ± 2.43 
    BMI (kg/m226.9 ± 3.9 26.8 ± 3.9 27.9 ± 4.4 
    Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) 143 ± 21 143 ± 21 150 ± 21 
    Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) 75 ± 11 75 ± 11 75 ± 11 
    HDL cholesterol (mmol/l) 1.39 ± 0.39 1.41 ± 0.39 1.26 ± 0.39 
    APOE ε4 carriership 915 (27) 838 (27) 77 (25) 
    Fasting insulin levels (mU/l)* 11.2 ± 7.8 10.9 ± 7.1 14.6 ± 12.4 
    Insulin resistance, HOMA* 3.07 ± 2.94 2.82 ± 2.25 5.58 ± 6.17 
Total sampleHistory of diabetes
NoYes
PROSPER    
    n 5,019 4,476 543 
    Age (years) 75.3 ± 3.3 75.3 ± 3.4 75.2 ± 3.2 
    Female sex 2,630 (52) 2,387 (53) 243 (45) 
    Low level of education 122 (2) 99 (2) 23 (4) 
    MMSE score (points) 28.1 ± 1.5 28.1 ± 1.5 28.0 ± 1.6 
    Fasting glucose levels (mmol/l) 5.45 ± 1.41 5.14 ± 0.78 8.02 ± 2.43 
    BMI (kg/m226.9 ± 4.2 26.7 ± 4.1 28.0 ± 4.3 
    Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) 155 ± 22 154 ± 22 158 ± 22 
    Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) 84 ± 11 84 ± 11 84 ± 11 
    HDL cholesterol (mmol/l) 1.28 ± 0.35 1.29 ± 0.35 1.17 ± 0.31 
    APOE ε4 carriership 1,192 (24) 1,091 (24) 101 (19) 
Rotterdam Study    
    n 3,428 3,116 312 
    Age (years) 71.8 ± 6.6 71.6 ± 6.5 73.8 ± 6.8 
    Female sex 1,975 (58) 1,817 (58) 158 (51) 
    Low level of education 515 (15) 472 (15) 43 (14) 
    MMSE score (points) 27.8 ± 1.8 27.8 ± 1.8 27.6 ± 1.9 
    Fasting glucose levels (mmol/l) 5.88 ± 1.35 5.65 ± 0.93 8.12 ± 2.43 
    BMI (kg/m226.9 ± 3.9 26.8 ± 3.9 27.9 ± 4.4 
    Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) 143 ± 21 143 ± 21 150 ± 21 
    Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) 75 ± 11 75 ± 11 75 ± 11 
    HDL cholesterol (mmol/l) 1.39 ± 0.39 1.41 ± 0.39 1.26 ± 0.39 
    APOE ε4 carriership 915 (27) 838 (27) 77 (25) 
    Fasting insulin levels (mU/l)* 11.2 ± 7.8 10.9 ± 7.1 14.6 ± 12.4 
    Insulin resistance, HOMA* 3.07 ± 2.94 2.82 ± 2.25 5.58 ± 6.17 

Data are means ± SD or n (%).

*Fasting insulin levels were available for 3,342 participants in the Rotterdam Study.

In PROSPER, fasting glucose levels were assessed during follow-up after 3, 6, 12, 24, and 36 months, in addition to the baseline assessment. In 3,491 participants without a history of diabetes, fasting glucose levels were available at baseline and after 36 months of follow-up. The mean glucose levels slightly increased over time. The quintiles of the mean fasting glucose ± SD at baseline and after 36 months were 4.30 ± 0.20 and 4.77 ± 0.67 mmol/l for the lowest quintile (quintile 1), 4.70 ± 0.08 and 4.95 ± 0.47 mmol/l for quintile 2, 5.00 ± 0.08 and 5.21 ± 0.67 mmol/l for quintile 3, 5.33 ± 0.11 and 5.46 ± 0.70 mmol/l for quintile 4, and 6.25 ± 0.83 and 6.30 ± 1.40 mmol/l for the highest quintile (quintile 5). In the Rotterdam sample, fasting glucose levels were assessed at the third survey as well as at the end of follow-up (fourth survey) in 2,209 participants without a history of diabetes. The quintiles of fasting glucose levels at the third survey and at the end of follow-up were 4.82 ± 0.24 and 5.07 ± 0.39 mmol/l for the lowest quintile (quintile 1), 5.20 ± 0.08 and 5.36 ± 0.42 mmol/l for quintile 2, 5.50 ± 0.08 and 5.55 ± 0.50 mmol/l for quintile 3, 5.84 ± 0.11 and 5.86 ± 0.57 mmol/l for quintile 4; and 6.73 ± .98 and 6.76 ± 1.44 mmol/l for the highest quintile (quintile 5).

Of the 8,447 participants who were present at baseline, 6,641 remained in the study sample until the end of follow-up, 985 withdrew from the study, and 821 died during follow-up. The 1,806 participants without a final examination comprised 20.8% of the participants without a history of diabetes and 26.3% of the participants with a history of diabetes at baseline (PROSPER) or at the third survey (Rotterdam Study).

Figure 1,A shows the relationship between fasting glucose levels and cognitive function at baseline for the 8,447 participants of the merged study sample. Study-specific quintiles of the distribution of fasting glucose levels in participants without a history of diabetes were constructed to account for the differences in fasting glucose levels between the studies. Cognitive test scores are shown for quintiles of the distribution of fasting glucose levels in participants without a history of diabetes and for participants with a history of diabetes. In participants without a history of diabetes, a rise in fasting glucose levels in the nondiabetes range was not associated with impairment in cognitive function, for any of the cognitive tests. Additionally, we compared the cognitive test scores at baseline for participants with and without a history of diabetes (Fig. 1 A) and showed that participants with a history of diabetes had worse cognitive function across the majority of tests at baseline when compared with participants without a history of diabetes (P < 0.05 for all tests except for the WFT in the Rotterdam Study sample).

FIG. 1.

A: Fasting glucose levels and cognitive function. z scores (SEM) for different cognitive test scores are plotted for study-specific quintiles of fasting glucose levels in nondiabetic participants (from lowest [quintile 1] to the highest [quintile 5] levels of fasting glucose) and for participants with a history of diabetes (DM). P values reflect the trend over the quintiles of fasting glucose levels, as well as the difference between participants without a history of diabetes (n = 7,592) and participants with a history of diabetes (n = 855). Estimates are based on the maximum number of participants available per cognitive test: global cognitive function (n = 8,447), WFT (n = 3,518), and 12-PLT (n = 5,223). B: Fasting glucose levels and change in cognitive function. z scores (SEM) represent annual change in cognitive test scores for study-specific quintiles of fasting glucose levels in nondiabetic participants (from lowest [quintile 1] to the highest [quintile 5] levels of fasting glucose) and for participants with a history of diabetes. P values reflect the trend over the quintiles of fasting glucose levels, as well as the difference between participants without a history of diabetes (n = 6,649) and participants with a history of diabetes (n = 719). Estimates are based on the maximum number of participants available per cognitive test: global cognitive function (n = 7,368), WFT (n = 2,639), and 12-PLT (n = 4,960). Linear mixed models were used, adjusted for age, sex, level of education, study (PROSPER or Rotterdam Study), BMI, HDL level, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, country, treatment group, and test version where applicable.

FIG. 1.

A: Fasting glucose levels and cognitive function. z scores (SEM) for different cognitive test scores are plotted for study-specific quintiles of fasting glucose levels in nondiabetic participants (from lowest [quintile 1] to the highest [quintile 5] levels of fasting glucose) and for participants with a history of diabetes (DM). P values reflect the trend over the quintiles of fasting glucose levels, as well as the difference between participants without a history of diabetes (n = 7,592) and participants with a history of diabetes (n = 855). Estimates are based on the maximum number of participants available per cognitive test: global cognitive function (n = 8,447), WFT (n = 3,518), and 12-PLT (n = 5,223). B: Fasting glucose levels and change in cognitive function. z scores (SEM) represent annual change in cognitive test scores for study-specific quintiles of fasting glucose levels in nondiabetic participants (from lowest [quintile 1] to the highest [quintile 5] levels of fasting glucose) and for participants with a history of diabetes. P values reflect the trend over the quintiles of fasting glucose levels, as well as the difference between participants without a history of diabetes (n = 6,649) and participants with a history of diabetes (n = 719). Estimates are based on the maximum number of participants available per cognitive test: global cognitive function (n = 7,368), WFT (n = 2,639), and 12-PLT (n = 4,960). Linear mixed models were used, adjusted for age, sex, level of education, study (PROSPER or Rotterdam Study), BMI, HDL level, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, country, treatment group, and test version where applicable.

Close modal

In the longitudinal analyses of the study population, there was no clear association between baseline fasting glucose levels and change in cognitive function during follow-up in participants without a history of diabetes (Fig. 1 B). In the PROSPER sample, higher levels of fasting glucose were associated with a decreased rate of decline on the 12-PLT (Ptrend = 0.039), but this was not seen for any of the other cognitive tests. Furthermore, participants with a history of diabetes did not show an increased rate of decline for any of the cognitive tests.

Additionally, we assessed the relationship between insulin resistance and cognitive function and decline for the 3,342 participants of the Rotterdam Study sample for whom fasting insulin levels were available (Fig. 2,A). The HOMA that was calculated for these participants was correlated with the fasting glucose levels (r = 0.54, P < 0.001), although the overlap between quintiles of the HOMA index and quintiles of fasting glucose levels was limited: only 35% of the participants without a history of diabetes were in the same quintile of the distribution for both fasting glucose and HOMA index. The relationship between insulin resistance and cognitive function was in accordance with findings on fasting glucose levels and cognitive function: in participants without a history of diabetes, rising insulin resistance was not associated with cognitive function. Similarly, there was no clear relationship between levels of insulin resistance and change in cognitive function during follow-up (Fig. 2 B). When the data from the PROSPER sample and the Rotterdam Study sample were analyzed separately, the findings were consistent with the results of the merged sample of 8,447 participants.

FIG. 2.

A: Insulin resistance (HOMA) and cognitive function in the Rotterdam Study. z scores (SEM) for different cognitive test scores are plotted for quintiles of insulin resistance (HOMA) in nondiabetic participants (from lowest [quintile 1] to the highest [quintile 5] levels of insulin resistance [HOMA]) and for participants with a history of diabetes (DM). P values reflect the trend over the quintiles of fasting glucose levels, as well as the difference between participants without a history of diabetes (n = 3,039) and participants with a history of diabetes (n = 303). Estimates are based on the maximum number of participants available per cognitive test: global cognitive function (n = 3,342) and WFT (n = 3,424). B: Insulin resistance (HOMA) and change in cognitive function. z scores (SEM) represent annual change in cognitive test scores for quintiles of insulin resistance (HOMA) in nondiabetic participants (from lowest [quintile 1] to the highest [quintile 5] levels of insulin resistance [HOMA]) and for participants with a history of diabetes. P values reflect the trend over the quintiles of fasting glucose levels, as well as the difference between participants without a history of diabetes (n = 2,331) and participants with a history of diabetes (n = 203). Estimates are based on the maximum number of participants available per cognitive test: global cognitive function (n = 2,534) and WFT (n = 2,567). Linear mixed models were used, adjusted for age, sex, level of education, BMI, HDL level, systolic blood pressure, and diastolic blood pressure.

FIG. 2.

A: Insulin resistance (HOMA) and cognitive function in the Rotterdam Study. z scores (SEM) for different cognitive test scores are plotted for quintiles of insulin resistance (HOMA) in nondiabetic participants (from lowest [quintile 1] to the highest [quintile 5] levels of insulin resistance [HOMA]) and for participants with a history of diabetes (DM). P values reflect the trend over the quintiles of fasting glucose levels, as well as the difference between participants without a history of diabetes (n = 3,039) and participants with a history of diabetes (n = 303). Estimates are based on the maximum number of participants available per cognitive test: global cognitive function (n = 3,342) and WFT (n = 3,424). B: Insulin resistance (HOMA) and change in cognitive function. z scores (SEM) represent annual change in cognitive test scores for quintiles of insulin resistance (HOMA) in nondiabetic participants (from lowest [quintile 1] to the highest [quintile 5] levels of insulin resistance [HOMA]) and for participants with a history of diabetes. P values reflect the trend over the quintiles of fasting glucose levels, as well as the difference between participants without a history of diabetes (n = 2,331) and participants with a history of diabetes (n = 203). Estimates are based on the maximum number of participants available per cognitive test: global cognitive function (n = 2,534) and WFT (n = 2,567). Linear mixed models were used, adjusted for age, sex, level of education, BMI, HDL level, systolic blood pressure, and diastolic blood pressure.

Close modal

Our study shows that, in an unprecedented large number of individuals from two independent prospective studies, higher levels of fasting glucose in the absence of a history of diabetes are not associated with cognitive function or cognitive decline. Furthermore, there was no association between insulin resistance (HOMA index) and cognitive function and decline in people without a history of diabetes. However, participants with a history of diabetes did have worse cognitive function at baseline than those without diabetes, although the magnitude of the observed effects was relatively small.

The results of our analyses do not fully correspond with previous findings in the Rotterdam Study, in which diabetes was found to be related to an increased risk of developing dementia (22). Although our data show that those with a history of diabetes have worse cognitive function at baseline than those without, one could argue that the effect sizes are lower than expected based on the previous report of an almost twofold increased risk of dementia for people with diabetes (22). It is possible that the measurement of cognitive function compared with the assessment of participants with dementia in the Rotterdam Study underlies this discrepancy. In the Rotterdam Study, we are able to continuously monitor the total cohort for incident dementia through computerized linkage between the study database and digitalized medical records from the general practitioners and the Regional Institute for Outpatient Mental Health Care. In contrast, in the Rotterdam sample, cognitive function assessments were performed with a 4.6-year interval, which could have led to selective nonparticipation of those in whom dementia was diagnosed between cognitive examinations (n = 85) and did not visit the research center for the follow-up assessment.

Furthermore, a previous report from the Nurses' Health Study showed that higher insulin levels in nondiabetic participants were related to faster cognitive decline (23). These results are not in agreement with our results on the relationship between insulin resistance and cognitive decline in the Rotterdam Study sample. The prolonged length of follow-up (>10 years compared with 4.6 years) in the Nurses' Health Study could have affected these results, as the effect of insulin resistance on cognitive function might be long-term. On the other hand, results from the ongoing Memory in Diabetes (MIND) substudy of the Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes (ACCORD) trial showed that higher A1C but not fasting plasma glucose levels was associated with worse cognitive function in a sample of almost 3,000 participants (24), which is in line with our results.

In individual participants, diabetes and impaired fasting glucose levels often co-occur with other cardiovascular risk factors. The metabolic syndrome is seen as a clustering of a number of these risk factors (abdominal obesity, hypertriglyceridemia, low HDL cholesterol, hypertension, and hyperglycemia) and is the subject of an ongoing discussion on the clinical use of the syndrome and its individual components in establishing the risk of cardiovascular disease and diabetes (25). Other studies have suggested a relationship between the metabolic syndrome and the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia (26,28). In our study, the clustering of these factors of the metabolic syndrome with the fasting glucose levels could have influenced our results. However, adjustment for BMI, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, and HDL cholesterol did not differ from the analyses that were unadjusted for these covariates.

Furthermore, the participants in PROSPER were included based on their increased cardiovascular risk profile: having either preexisting vascular disease or increased risk of such disease due to a history of smoking, hypertension, or diabetes. The known association between these cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive function and decline might have interfered with our investigation of the relationship between fasting glucose levels and cognitive function and decline. However, when we excluded the 2,823 participants in PROSPER who had a history of vascular disease from the sample of 5,019 participants, the relationship between fasting glucose levels and cognitive function at baseline did not markedly differ from our findings in the total sample: there was no clear relationship between fasting glucose levels and cognitive function in participants without a history of vascular disease. Moreover, after exclusion of the 2,823 participants, there was no significant relationship anymore between history of diabetes and baseline global cognitive function. The relationship between fasting glucose levels and change in cognitive function did not markedly change after exclusion of the participants with a history of vascular disease. The same was seen for the analyses on the relationship between history of diabetes and change in cognitive function. Therefore, we do not think that the inclusion of participants with preexisting vascular disease in PROSPER has masked a possible association between fasting glucose and insulin resistance on cognitive function.

We used fasting insulin levels that were available for almost the entire sample of the Rotterdam Study to calculate the HOMA index as a measure of insulin resistance to further investigate the relationship between glucose metabolism and cognitive function. However, the relationship between insulin resistance and cognitive function and decline showed similarities with the association between fasting glucose levels and cognitive function and decline: in participants without a history of diabetes, insulin resistance was not associated with cognitive function or decline.

Previous population-based studies that investigated the relationship between glucose metabolism and cognitive functions suggested a number of possible biological mechanisms that could be involved, ranging from accumulation of advanced glycation end products (2) and accelerated cerebrovascular disease (1) to the role of the insulin-degrading enzyme on amyloid β metabolism (3). Although it is difficult to address the role of these suggested mechanisms, our study of >8,000 participants shows that the effect of increased fasting glucose levels on cognitive function seems to be long-term and independent of other cardiovascular risk factors like BMI, blood pressure, and HDL cholesterol levels.

The observed differences in cognitive test scores between people with and without a history of diabetes were relatively moderate and may therefore lack clinical significance for individuals. However, small effect sizes do not automatically imply irrelevance of the observed effect, as small effects on the group level can indeed represent large effects for a number of participants.

In the analyses of the annual decline in cognitive function, the PROSPER sample failed to show a clear decline in MMSE score over time, although this was seen in the Rotterdam sample with participants of comparable age. It is possible that a potential learning effect of the MMSE had a higher impact on the PROSPER sample compared with the Rotterdam Study sample because of the shorter time span between cognitive measurements (19). Additionally, the selection criteria for participants in PROSPER (baseline MMSE score ≥ 24) may have resulted in a sample of participants with slightly better cognitive function, which is also represented in the difference in MMSE scores of both samples at baseline and might have had an effect on the annual decline of MMSE score that was measured in PROSPER.

The strengths of this study consist of the prospective design, the large number of participants in both studies, and the dedicated neuropsychological test battery that was used in both samples. Furthermore, we had the possibility of studying the variability of fasting glucose levels during follow-up and of examining the appropriateness of using a single measurement of fasting glucose level to assess the association between fasting glucose levels and cognitive function and decline. A large variation in fasting glucose levels over time could have disturbed our analyses through the phenomenon of “regression-to-the-mean.” However, the levels of fasting glucose during follow-up did not materially differ from the baseline or third survey in both study samples. Therefore, we decided to use the baseline or third survey fasting glucose measurement in our analyses.

Some limitations need to be addressed. Participants who were present at baseline but did not undergo follow-up examinations were predominantly present in the group with a history of diabetes. They had worse cognitive function at baseline compared with the participants who stayed in the study until the end of follow-up. This selective attrition of participants with relatively high levels of fasting glucose and concurrent low levels of cognitive function could have resulted in an underestimation of our estimates of cognitive decline for participants with a history of diabetes. We also recognize that some individuals with diabetes would have been missed because of lack of oral glucose tolerance testing. More importantly, undiagnosed diabetes would be more prevalent in those in the higher quintiles for fasting glucose and would have biased the study toward an association of higher quintiles and cognitive decline, not the other way around. Thus, lack of oral glucose tolerance testing does not negate our findings; rather it gives us added confidence that our observations are valid.

In conclusion, elevations in fasting glucose levels are not clearly associated with impaired cognitive function or with an accelerated rate of cognitive decline in participants without a history of diabetes. Furthermore, there was no clear relationship between insulin resistance (HOMA index) and cognitive function and decline in participants without a history of diabetes. These data suggest that cognitive decline accelerates strongly once a person is diabetic but not with lesser degrees of dysglycemia. As a result, preventing individuals at risk from developing diabetes through lifestyle changes may also lead to large societal gains by preventing such individuals from undergoing accelerated cognitive decline.

The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

The Rotterdam Study is supported by the Erasmus Medical Center and Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO), the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development, the Research Institute for Diseases in the Elderly (RIDE), the Ministry of Education, Culture and Science, the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sports, the European Commission (DG XII), and the Municipality of Rotterdam. The work in PROSPER was supported by an unrestricted investigator-initiated grant from Bristol-Myers Squibb, USA. This study was supported by the NWO, by RIDE grant 948-00-021, by the Alzheimer's Association grant IIRG-06-27261, and by the International Foundation Alzheimer's Research.

No potential conflicts of interest relevant to this article were reported.

Author contributions: N.S. contributed to acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of the data, drafting of the manuscript, and critical revision of the manuscript. J.W. and A.H. contributed to study concept and design, acquisition of data, and critical revision of the manuscript. E.B. and I.P. contributed to study concept and design and critical revision of the manuscript. E.S. contributed to acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of the data, and critical revision of the manuscript. M.B. and R.W. contributed to study concept and design, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of the data, drafting of the manuscript, and critical revision of the manuscript. S.M. contributed to acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of the data, and drafting of the manuscript.

1.
Biessels
GJ
,
Staekenborg
S
,
Brunner
E
,
Brayne
C
,
Scheltens
P
:
Risk of dementia in diabetes mellitus: a systematic review
.
Lancet Neurol
2006
;
5
:
64
74
2.
Münch
G
,
Schinzel
R
,
Loske
C
,
Wong
A
,
Durany
N
,
Li
JJ
,
Vlassara
H
,
Smith
MA
,
Perry
G
,
Riederer
P
:
Alzheimer's disease—synergistic effects of glucose deficit, oxidative stress and advanced glycation endproducts
.
J Neural Transm
1998
;
105
:
439
461
3.
Craft
S
:
Insulin resistance syndrome and Alzheimer's disease: age- and obesity-related effects on memory, amyloid, and inflammation
.
Neurobiol Aging
2005
;
26
(
Suppl. 1
):
65
69
4.
Lindahl
B
,
Weinehall
L
,
Asplund
K
,
Hallmans
G
:
Screening for impaired glucose tolerance. Results from a population-based study in 21,057 individuals
.
Diabetes Care
1999
;
22
:
1988
1992
5.
Expert Committee on the Diagnosis and Classification of Diabetes Mellitus. Report of the expert committee on the diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus
.
Diabetes Care
2003
;
26
(
Suppl. 1
):
S5
S20
6.
American Diabetes Association
.
Diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus
.
Diabetes Care
2005
;
28
(
Suppl. 1
):
S37
S42
7.
Fontbonne
A
,
Berr
C
,
Ducimetière
P
,
Alpérovitch
A
:
Changes in cognitive abilities over a 4-year period are unfavorably affected in elderly diabetic subjects: results of the Epidemiology of Vascular Aging Study
.
Diabetes Care
2001
;
24
:
366
370
8.
Kanaya
AM
,
Barrett-Connor
E
,
Gildengorin
G
,
Yaffe
K
:
Change in cognitive function by glucose tolerance status in older adults: a 4-year prospective study of the Rancho Bernardo Study Cohort
.
Arch Intern Med
2004
;
164
:
1327
1333
9.
Fuh
JL
,
Wang
SJ
,
Hwu
CM
,
Lu
SR
:
Glucose tolerance status and cognitive impairment in early middle-aged women
.
Diabet Med
2007
;
24
:
788
791
10.
Abbatecola
AM
,
Paolisso
G
,
Lamponi
M
,
Bandinelli
S
,
Lauretani
F
,
Launer
L
,
Ferrucci
L
:
Insulin resistance and executive dysfunction in older persons
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
2004
;
52
:
1713
1718
11.
Craft
S
:
Insulin resistance and Alzheimer's disease pathogenesis: potential mechanisms and implications for treatment
.
Current Alzheimer Research
2007
;
4
:
147
152
12.
Shepherd
J
,
Blauw
GJ
,
Murphy
MB
,
Cobbe
SM
,
Bollen
EL
,
Buckley
BM
,
Ford
I
,
Jukema
JW
,
Hyland
M
,
Gaw
A
,
Lagaay
AM
,
Perry
IJ
,
Macfarlane
PW
,
Meinders
AE
,
Sweeney
BJ
,
Packard
CJ
,
Westendorp
RG
,
Twomey
C
,
Stott
DJ
:
The design of a PROspective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk (PROSPER)
.
Am J Cardiol
1999
;
84
:
1192
1197
13.
Shepherd
J
,
Blauw
GJ
,
Murphy
MB
,
Bollen
EL
,
Buckley
BM
,
Cobbe
SM
,
Ford
I
,
Gaw
A
,
Hyland
M
,
Jukema
JW
,
Kamper
AM
,
Macfarlane
PW
,
Meinders
AE
,
Norrie
J
,
Packard
CJ
,
Perry
IJ
,
Stott
DJ
,
Sweeney
BJ
,
Twomey
C
,
Westendorp
RG
:
PROspective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk. Pravastatin in elderly individuals at risk of vascular disease (PROSPER): a randomised controlled trial
.
Lancet
2002
;
360
:
1623
1630
14.
Hofman
A
,
Breteler
MM
,
van Duijn
CM
,
Janssen
HL
,
Krestin
GP
,
Kuipers
EJ
,
Stricker
BH
,
Tiemeier
H
,
Uitterlinden
AG
,
Vingerling
JR
,
Witteman
JC
:
The Rotterdam Study: 2010 objectives and design update
.
Eur J Epidemiol
2009
;
24
:
553
572
15.
Folstein
MF
,
Folstein
SE
,
McHugh
PR
:
“Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician
.
J Psychiatr Res
1975
;
12
:
189
198
16.
Lezak
MD
,
Howieson
DB
,
Loring
DW
:
Neuropsychological Assessment
. 4th ed.
New York, NY
,
Oxford University Press
,
2004
17.
Golden
CJ
:
Identification of brain disorders by the Stroop Color and Word Test
.
J Clin Psychol
1976
;
32
:
654
658
18.
Welsh
KA
,
Butters
N
,
Mohs
RC
,
Beekly
D
,
Edland
S
,
Fillenbaum
G
,
Heyman
A
:
The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part V. A normative study of the neuropsychological battery
.
Neurology
1994
;
44
:
609
614
19.
Houx
PJ
,
Shepherd
J
,
Blauw
GJ
,
Murphy
MB
,
Ford
I
,
Bollen
EL
,
Buckley
B
,
Stott
DJ
,
Jukema
W
,
Hyland
M
,
Gaw
A
,
Norrie
J
,
Kamper
AM
,
Perry
IJ
,
MacFarlane
PW
,
Meinders
AE
,
Sweeney
BJ
,
Packard
CJ
,
Twomey
C
,
Cobbe
SM
,
Westendorp
RG
:
Testing cognitive function in elderly populations: the PROSPER study. PROspective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
2002
;
73
:
385
389
20.
Matthews
DR
,
Hosker
JP
,
Rudenski
AS
,
Naylor
BA
,
Treacher
DF
,
Turner
RC
:
Homeostasis model assessment: insulin resistance and beta-cell function from fasting plasma glucose and insulin concentrations in man
.
Diabetologia
1985
;
28
:
412
419
21.
Bonora
E
,
Targher
G
,
Alberiche
M
,
Bonadonna
RC
,
Saggiani
F
,
Zenere
MB
,
Monauni
T
,
Muggeo
M
:
Homeostasis model assessment closely mirrors the glucose clamp technique in the assessment of insulin sensitivity: studies in subjects with various degrees of glucose tolerance and insulin sensitivity
.
Diabetes Care
2000
;
23
:
57
63
22.
Ott
A
,
Stolk
RP
,
van Harskamp
F
,
Pols
HA
,
Hofman
A
,
Breteler
MM
:
Diabetes mellitus and the risk of dementia: the Rotterdam Study
.
Neurology
1999
;
53
:
1937
1942
23.
van Oijen
M
,
Okereke
OI
,
Kang
JH
,
Pollak
MN
,
Hu
FB
,
Hankinson
SE
,
Grodstein
F
:
Fasting insulin levels and cognitive decline in older women without diabetes
.
Neuroepidemiology
2008
;
30
:
174
179
24.
Cukierman-Yaffe
T
,
Gerstein
HC
,
Williamson
JD
,
Lazar
RM
,
Lovato
L
,
Miller
ME
,
Coker
LH
,
Murray
A
,
Sullivan
MD
,
Marcovina
SM
,
Launer
LJ
:
Relationship between baseline glycemic control and cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes and other cardiovascular risk factors. The Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes-Memory in Diabetes (ACCORD-MIND) trial
.
Diabetes Care
2009
;
32
:
221
226
25.
Sattar
N
,
McConnachie
A
,
Shaper
AG
,
Blauw
GJ
,
Buckley
BM
,
de Craen
AJ
,
Ford
I
,
Forouhi
NG
,
Freeman
DJ
,
Jukema
JW
,
Lennon
L
,
Macfarlane
PW
,
Murphy
MB
,
Packard
CJ
,
Stott
DJ
,
Westendorp
RG
,
Whincup
PH
,
Shepherd
J
,
Wannamethee
SG
:
Can metabolic syndrome usefully predict cardiovascular disease and diabetes? Outcome data from two prospective studies
.
Lancet
2008
;
371
:
1927
1935
26.
Vanhanen
M
,
Koivisto
K
,
Moilanen
L
,
Helkala
EL
,
Hänninen
T
,
Soininen
H
,
Kervinen
K
,
Kesäniemi
YA
,
Laakso
M
,
Kuusisto
J
:
Association of metabolic syndrome with Alzheimer disease: a population-based study
.
Neurology
2006
;
67
:
843
847
27.
Yaffe
K
,
Kanaya
A
,
Lindquist
K
,
Simonsick
EM
,
Harris
T
,
Shorr
RI
,
Tylavsky
FA
,
Newman
AB
:
The metabolic syndrome, inflammation, and risk of cognitive decline
.
JAMA
2004
;
292
:
2237
2242
28.
Kalmijn
S
,
Foley
D
,
White
L
,
Burchfiel
CM
,
Curb
JD
,
Petrovitch
H
,
Ross
GW
,
Havlik
RJ
,
Launer
LJ
:
Metabolic cardiovascular syndrome and risk of dementia in Japanese-American elderly men. The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study
.
Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol
2000
;
20
:
2255
2260
Readers may use this article as long as the work is properly cited, the use is educational and not for profit, and the work is not altered. See http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/ for details.

Supplementary data