Aims

The aims of this study were to assess domains of executive function in relation to diabetes management and glycemic control in adolescents with type 1 diabetes and to compare adolescent self-report and parent proxy-report of adolescent executive function.

Methods

Adolescents with type 1 diabetes (N = 169, 46% female, age 15.9 ± 1.3 years) and their parents completed self-report and parent proxy-report versions of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF).

Results

Self-report and parent proxy-report BRIEF T scores were moderately to strongly correlated; parent proxy scores were significantly higher than self-report scores. Executive function problems (Global Executive Composite T score ≥60) occurred in 9% of adolescents by self-report and 26% by parent proxy-report. For almost all Metacognition Index scales, elevated (T score ≥60) parent proxy scores were associated with lower adherence, lower adolescent diabetes self-efficacy, and more parent involvement in diabetes management. Elevated scores on several Metacognition Index scales were associated with less pump use (Plan/Organize by self-report, Initiate by parent proxy-report, and Monitor by parent proxy-report) and higher A1C (Plan/Organize by self-report and parent proxy-report and Organization of Materials by parent proxy-report). The only significant associations for the Behavioral Regulation Index scales occurred for adherence (by parent proxy-report) and diabetes self-efficacy (by self-report and parent-report).

Conclusion

Adolescents with type 1 diabetes who have problems with metacognition may need additional support for diabetes self-management.

Executive function refers to a set of cognitive processes involving emotion regulation and goal-directed, problem-solving behaviors such as planning, organization, attention, initiation, inhibition, and working memory (1,2). Development of executive function begins in early childhood and continues throughout adolescence and into young adulthood (3). Adolescence is also a time of increasing independence from parents and family. For adolescents with type 1 diabetes, responsibility for daily diabetes management shifts from parents to adolescents. Diabetes management is multifaceted and involves checking glucose levels, estimating carbohydrate intake, determining insulin doses, and administering insulin, while considering factors such as exercise and acute illnesses. Suboptimal executive function can create additional challenges to diabetes self-care in adolescents (4). Previous studies in adolescents with type 1 diabetes have found that poorer executive function is associated with poorer diabetes management adherence and higher A1C (59).

The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) (10) has been used frequently in research settings to assess executive function of children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes. The BRIEF provides a parent proxy-report of adolescent executive function (a teacher version also available). The BRIEF-Self-Report (BRIEF-SR) is a companion self-report version, validated in youth aged 11–18 years (11). The BRIEF-Parent (86 items) and BRIEF-SR (80 items) are composed of eight clinical scales measuring specific aspects of executive function, two summary indices (the Behavioral Regulation Index [BRI] and the Metacognition Index [MI]), and an overall score (Global Executive Composite [GEC]).

Although the BRIEF has been widely used in adolescents with type 1 diabetes, many of these studies have reported T scores for only the GEC or selected scales (5,12,13). The clinical scales provide a detailed assessment about specific areas of executive function problems that may create difficulties with diabetes management for adolescents. The BRI scales measure a person’s abilities to appropriately inhibit thoughts and actions, shift attention between tasks or topics, solve problems in a flexible manner, and modulate emotional responses. Adolescents with type 1 diabetes who have problems with behavioral regulation may have difficulty stopping an activity to check a glucose level or administer insulin. They may also become extremely frustrated or exhibit overly emotional reactions to out-of-range glucose levels.

The MI scales measure an individuals’ abilities in areas such as planning and initiating an activity, generating problem-solving strategies, holding information in working memory, and maintaining organization in their environment. Adolescents with type 1 diabetes who have problems with metacognition may have difficulties keeping track of supplies or gathering the information needed to calculate an insulin dose. They may also have difficulties troubleshooting problems with insulin pumps or continuous glucose monitoring (CGM) systems.

In this study, we aimed to assess whether specific domains of executive function (BRIEF clinical scales) have differing relationships with various aspects of diabetes management and glycemic control. We also sought to provide detailed descriptive data (mean and SD of T scores) for the GEC, BRI, MI, and all clinical scales for the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-SR in a contemporary sample of adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Few studies in adolescents with type 1 diabetes have used both the BRIEF-Parent and the BRIEF-SR (9,14). Because BRIEF-SR scores are generally lower than BRIEF-Parent scores (11), it is difficult to compare data across multiple studies using different raters (i.e., adolescent self-report in one study vs. parent proxy-report in another study). The report of BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-SR data from a single sample provides a valuable source for comparison by researchers using the BRIEF-Parent and/or BRIEF-SR in other samples of adolescents with type 1 diabetes or other chronic health conditions.

Participants

Study participants were 169 adolescents with type 1 diabetes and 168 parents/guardians (hereafter referred to as parents) of the adolescents. One parent was excluded from analyses because of too many missing responses on the measure of executive function. The participants were enrolled in a longitudinal study aimed at improving self-care and glycemic outcomes in adolescents with type 1 diabetes; results from the entire study have been previously published (15).

Participants in the current report agreed to participate in an ancillary nonintervention study involving assessment of adolescent executive function. The primary aim of the ancillary study was to assess the relationship between executive function and diabetes management and glycemic control in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. The ancillary study was conducted at one of the two sites involved in the longitudinal study. All of the site’s participants agreed to participate in the ancillary study.

Data were collected from January 2015 to March 2016. Eligibility criteria for the main study included: age 13–17 years, type 1 diabetes duration ≥6 months, daily insulin dose ≥0.5 units/kg, A1C 6.5–11.0%, fluency in English (for completion of surveys), clinical care at the study site, no significant developmental or cognitive disorder that would prevent full study participation, and no significant mental illness (i.e., major psychiatric disorder or inpatient psychiatric admission in the previous 6 months). The institutional review board at the Joslin Diabetes Center approved the ancillary study protocol, and adolescents/parents provided written informed assent/consent before completing any study procedures.

Executive Function Data

The BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-SR were used to assess adolescents’ executive function (10,11). Parents completed the 86-item BRIEF-Parent form, which has been validated in parents of youth aged 5–18 years. Adolescents completed the 80-item BRIEF-SR, which has been validated in youth aged 11–18 years. Each item on the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-SR describes a behavior for which the respondent rates the degree to which it has been a problem in the past 6 months by selecting the rating “never,” “sometimes,” or “often.” Examples include “I have problems getting started on my own” and “I react more strongly to situations than my friends.” The BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-SR are each composed of eight clinical scales assessing specific aspects of executive function: Emotional Control, Inhibit, Shift, Monitor, Organization of Materials, Plan/Organize, Initiate (BRIEF-Parent only), Task Completion (BRIEF-SR only), and Working Memory. The clinical scales are summarized in the BRI and MI, and overall executive function is represented by the GEC, which includes all clinical scales.

The BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-SR were completed on paper, and trained research staff double data-entered responses into the BRIEF Software Portfolio (PAR, Inc.), which calculates age- and sex-adjusted T scores for the GEC, BRI, MI, and clinical scales. T scores can range from 30 to 100, with a mean of 50 and an SD of 10. Higher T scores indicate poorer executive function. T scores ≥60 are considered mildly elevated, and those ≥65 are considered clinically elevated.

Two parents scored at the cutoff score on the BRIEF validity scales—one on the Inconsistency Scale and the other on the Negativity Scale. After careful review of both parents’ responses, neither was determined to be invalid; therefore, both were retained in the data set. None of the adolescents scored at or above the cutoff score on the validity scales.

Medical, Diabetes, and Demographic Data

Medical and diabetes treatment information was collected by parent/adolescent interview and medical record review on the same day the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-SR were completed. Demographic data (e.g., race/ethnicity and parental education level) were obtained from a survey completed by parents. Diagnosis of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) was obtained from parent/adolescent reports. A1C was measured by immunoturbidimetric methodology using the Roche Cobas Integra assay (reference range 4.0–6.0%; Roche Diagnostics, Indianapolis, IN). Adolescents and parents also completed the following surveys on tablet computers using REDCap software (16).

Diabetes Management Questionnaire

The Diabetes Management Questionnaire (17) measures adherence to diabetes care tasks over the past month. Adolescents and parents completed parallel versions of the survey. The survey has 20 items (e.g., “How often did you or your parent/guardian check your blood sugar before physical activity?”), answered on a five-point Likert scale with answers ranging from “almost never” to “almost always.” Total scores range from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating better adherence. In the study sample, the Cronbach α was 0.84 for adolescents and 0.82 for parents.

Diabetes Management Self-Efficacy Survey

The Diabetes Management Self-Efficacy Survey (18) measures an adolescent’s confidence in completing diabetes management tasks in that moment (i.e., “right now”). Adolescents and parents completed parallel versions of the survey; adolescents rated their own diabetes self-efficacy, and parents rated their child’s diabetes self-efficacy. The survey has 16 items (e.g., “I am sure that I can remember to inject or bolus my insulin before eating”), answered on a five-point Likert scale with answers ranging from “disagree a lot” to “agree a lot.” Total scores range from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating higher diabetes self-efficacy. In the study sample, the Cronbach α was 0.90 for adolescents and 0.94 for parents.

Diabetes Family Responsibility Questionnaire

The Diabetes Family Responsibility Questionnaire (19) measures parental involvement in diabetes management tasks over the past month. Adolescents and parents completed parallel versions of the survey. The survey has 19 items (e.g., “Giving insulin injections or boluses [pump]”), for which adolescents and parents each rated who had primary responsibility for carrying out that diabetes task, with answer options of “child,” “equal,” and “parent.” Total scores range from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating more parental involvement. In the study sample, the Cronbach α was 0.80 for adolescents and 0.85 for parents.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were performed with SAS, v. 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent T scores were compared using paired t tests and Pearson correlations (20). For both the BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent, we calculated the percentage of adolescents with at least mild problems (T score ≥60) and the percentage with clinically significant problems (T score ≥65) in executive function. McNemar’s test was used to assess agreement between elevated scores on the BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent. Fisher exact, t, and χ2 tests were used to examine associations between demographic and diabetes characteristics and executive function problems (T score ≥60). Because mild problems with executive function may affect diabetes management, we used the cutoff score of 60, which has been used in previous reports (21), for these analyses. Because of the number of comparisons, P <0.01 was considered statistically significant.

Participant Characteristics

Participant characteristics and diabetes survey scores for the 169 adolescents (46% female) and 168 parents (83% mothers) are shown in Table 1. The mean age was 15.9 ± 1.3 years, and the mean diabetes duration was 8.4 ± 3.7 years. The sample was relatively homogeneous: 88% were non-Hispanic White, 72% had a parent with at least a college degree, and 67% were using an insulin pump. The mean A1C was 8.5 ± 1.2%, and only 16% had an A1C <7.5%. Fourteen percent self-reported a diagnosis of ADHD. Adolescents’ and parents’ reports of diabetes adherence were similar (Table 1). Adolescents’ ratings of diabetes self-efficacy were significantly higher than those of parents (P <0.0001), and adolescents reported significantly less parental involvement in diabetes management than did parents (P <0.0001).

Table 1

Participant Characteristics

CharacteristicMean ± SD or %
Age, years 15.9 ± 1.3 
Female sex 46 
Non-Hispanic White race 88 
Parent education
Less than college degree
College degree
Graduate school degree 

28
33
39 
ADHD 14 
Diabetes duration, years 8.4 ± 3.7 
Daily insulin dose, units/kg 0.97 ± 0.28 
Insulin pump use 67 
Blood glucose monitoring frequency, times/day 4.5 ± 2.1 
CGM use 21 
A1C, % 8.5 ± 1.2 
Diabetes adherence
Adolescent report
Parent report 

68 ± 15
68 ± 14 
Diabetes self-efficacy
Adolescent report
Parent report 

82 ± 16
72 ± 22 
Diabetes parent involvement
Adolescent report
Parent report 

36 ± 13
44 ± 14 
CharacteristicMean ± SD or %
Age, years 15.9 ± 1.3 
Female sex 46 
Non-Hispanic White race 88 
Parent education
Less than college degree
College degree
Graduate school degree 

28
33
39 
ADHD 14 
Diabetes duration, years 8.4 ± 3.7 
Daily insulin dose, units/kg 0.97 ± 0.28 
Insulin pump use 67 
Blood glucose monitoring frequency, times/day 4.5 ± 2.1 
CGM use 21 
A1C, % 8.5 ± 1.2 
Diabetes adherence
Adolescent report
Parent report 

68 ± 15
68 ± 14 
Diabetes self-efficacy
Adolescent report
Parent report 

82 ± 16
72 ± 22 
Diabetes parent involvement
Adolescent report
Parent report 

36 ± 13
44 ± 14 

BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent Scores

BRIEF-Parent T scores were significantly higher than BRIEF-SR scores for the GEC, BRI, MI, and all clinical scales (all P <0.0001) (Table 2). Nonetheless, BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent scores were moderately to strongly correlated on the GEC (R = 0.62), BRI (R = 0.61), MI (R = 0.59), and clinical scales (R = 0.47–0.60) (all P <0.0001).

Table 2

T Scores by Adolescent Self-Report (BRIEF-SR) and Parent Proxy-Report (BRIEF-Parent)

Survey ComponentBRIEF-SR (N = 169)BRIEF-Parent (N = 168)Difference (95% CI)*Correlation Coefficient (R)*
GEC 45.09 ± 10.41 52.68 ± 11.37 7.68 (6.22–9.13) 0.62 
BRI
Inhibit
Shift
Emotional Control
Monitor 
44.24 ± 9.82
44.22 ± 9.04
45.36 ± 10.14
47.17 ± 10.50
43.66 ± 8.22 
50.57 ± 10.38
48.70 ± 8.92
51.32 ± 11.41
51.51 ± 10.94
— 
6.40 (5.04–7.76)
4.52 (3.23–5.80)
6.06 (4.49–7.63)
4.40 (2.89–5.91)
— 
0.61
0.56
0.55
0.57
— 
MI
Initiate
Working Memory
Plan/Organize
Organization of Materials
Task Completion
Monitor 
46.45 ± 10.65

46.07 ± 9.99
46.10 ± 9.93
47.87 ± 10.01
48.16 ± 10.53
— 
53.74 ± 11.60
53.00 ± 11.71
54.36 ± 12.67
52.70 ± 11.07
54.48 ± 10.85

50.89 ± 10.73 
7.38 (5.85–8.91)

8.33 (6.74–9.92)
6.65 (5.00–8.31)
6.76 (5.14–8.37)

— 
0.59

0.60
0.47
0.48

— 
Survey ComponentBRIEF-SR (N = 169)BRIEF-Parent (N = 168)Difference (95% CI)*Correlation Coefficient (R)*
GEC 45.09 ± 10.41 52.68 ± 11.37 7.68 (6.22–9.13) 0.62 
BRI
Inhibit
Shift
Emotional Control
Monitor 
44.24 ± 9.82
44.22 ± 9.04
45.36 ± 10.14
47.17 ± 10.50
43.66 ± 8.22 
50.57 ± 10.38
48.70 ± 8.92
51.32 ± 11.41
51.51 ± 10.94
— 
6.40 (5.04–7.76)
4.52 (3.23–5.80)
6.06 (4.49–7.63)
4.40 (2.89–5.91)
— 
0.61
0.56
0.55
0.57
— 
MI
Initiate
Working Memory
Plan/Organize
Organization of Materials
Task Completion
Monitor 
46.45 ± 10.65

46.07 ± 9.99
46.10 ± 9.93
47.87 ± 10.01
48.16 ± 10.53
— 
53.74 ± 11.60
53.00 ± 11.71
54.36 ± 12.67
52.70 ± 11.07
54.48 ± 10.85

50.89 ± 10.73 
7.38 (5.85–8.91)

8.33 (6.74–9.92)
6.65 (5.00–8.31)
6.76 (5.14–8.37)

— 
0.59

0.60
0.47
0.48

— 

Data are mean ± SD.

*

All P <0.0001.

The Monitor scale is part of the BRI in the BRIEF-SR and part of the MI in the BRIEF-Parent.

The percentages of elevated BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent scores are shown in Table 3. Four percent of adolescents had clinically significant executive function problems (GEC ≥65) by BRIEF-SR, compared with 16% by BRIEF-Parent (P <0.0001). Nine percent of adolescents had at least mild executive function problems (GEC ≥60) by BRIEF-SR, compared with 26% by BRIEF-Parent (P <0.0001). The percentage of adolescents with T scores ≥60 on the BRI, MI, and clinical scales ranged from 5 to 17% on the BRIEF-SR and 15–35% on the BRIEF-Parent. Scores on the GEC, BRI, MI, and clinical scales were more likely to be elevated (≥60) by parent proxy-report than by adolescent self-report (P <0.0001 for GEC, MI, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, and Organization of Materials; P = 0.0002 for BRI; P = 0.0003 for Shift and Emotional Control; and P = 0.003 for Inhibit).

Table 3

Percentages of T Scores ≥65 and ≥60 by Adolescent Self-Report (BRIEF-SR) and Parent Proxy-Report (BRIEF-Parent)

Survey ComponentBRIEF-SR (N = 169)BRIEF-Parent (N = 168)
T Score ≥65T Score ≥60T Score ≥65T Score ≥60
GEC 16 26 
BRI
Inhibit
Shift
Emotional Control
Monitor* 
4
4
6
7
8
7
11
10
12
7
17
14
— 
18
15
23
22
— 
MI
Initiate
Working Memory
Plan/Organize
Organization of Materials
Task Completion
Monitor* 
7

6
5
8
8
— 
10

10
7
11
17
— 
18
17
19
17
24

14 
27
25
30
24
35

24 
Survey ComponentBRIEF-SR (N = 169)BRIEF-Parent (N = 168)
T Score ≥65T Score ≥60T Score ≥65T Score ≥60
GEC 16 26 
BRI
Inhibit
Shift
Emotional Control
Monitor* 
4
4
6
7
8
7
11
10
12
7
17
14
— 
18
15
23
22
— 
MI
Initiate
Working Memory
Plan/Organize
Organization of Materials
Task Completion
Monitor* 
7

6
5
8
8
— 
10

10
7
11
17
— 
18
17
19
17
24

14 
27
25
30
24
35

24 
*

The Monitor scale is part of the BRI in the BRIEF-SR and part of the MI in the BRIEF-Parent.

Neither elevated BRIEF-SR T scores nor elevated BRIEF-Parent T scores on the GEC, BRI, or MI were significantly associated with adolescent sex, age, age at diabetes diagnosis, or diabetes duration. Racial/ethnic minorities were more likely to have BRI T scores ≥60 on the BRIEF-Parent compared with non-Hispanic White adolescents (45 vs. 15%, P = 0.003). Adolescents with self-reported ADHD were more likely than adolescents without ADHD to have GEC T scores ≥60 on the BRIEF-Parent (71 vs. 18%, P <0.0001) and MI T scores ≥60 on both the BRIEF-SR (29 vs. 7%, P = 0.004) and BRIEF-Parent (79 vs. 18%, P <0.0001).

We examined the level of concordance between BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent reports of executive function problems. Concordance was defined as both BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent <60 or both ≥60. Discordance was defined as BRIEF-SR <60 and BRIEF-Parent ≥60 or BRIEF-SR ≥60 and BRIEF-Parent <60. Concordance between BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent T scores was 81% on the GEC, 85% on the BRI (80–85% on BRI clinical scales), and 78% on the MI (70–79% on MI clinical scales) (Figure 1). Discordance resulting from an elevated BRIEF-Parent score without an elevated BRIEF-SR score was 18% on the GEC, 13% on the BRI (12–16% on BRI clinical scales), and 20% on the MI (19–27% on MI clinical scales). Discordance resulting from an elevated BRIEF-SR score without an elevated BRIEF-Parent score was low, ranging from 1 to 4% on the GEC, BRI, MI, and clinical scales.

FIGURE 1

Concordance/discordance in adolescent executive function problems by adolescent self-report (BRIEF-SR) and parent proxy-report (BRIEF-Parent). T scores ≥60 indicate at least mild executive function problems. EC, Emotional Control; INHIB, Inhibit; OM, Organization of Materials; PO, Plan/Organize; WM, Working Memory.

FIGURE 1

Concordance/discordance in adolescent executive function problems by adolescent self-report (BRIEF-SR) and parent proxy-report (BRIEF-Parent). T scores ≥60 indicate at least mild executive function problems. EC, Emotional Control; INHIB, Inhibit; OM, Organization of Materials; PO, Plan/Organize; WM, Working Memory.

Close modal

Associations Between Elevated (≥60) BRIEF-SR and BRIEF-Parent T Scores and Diabetes Characteristics

GEC, BRI, and MI

Elevated BRIEF-SR scores on the GEC and BRI were associated with lower self-reports of diabetes self-efficacy (GEC P = 0.008, BRI P = 0.007). There were no other significant associations between elevations on the BRIEF-SR GEC, BRI, and MI and diabetes characteristics. Problems with executive function by parent proxy-report were associated with several diabetes characteristics (Table 4). Elevated BRIEF-Parent scores on the GEC, BRI, and/or MI were associated with lower rates of insulin pump use (GEC P = 0.001, BRI P = 0.0005, MI P = 0.0008), higher A1C (GEC P = 0.008, BRI P = 0.002, MI P = 0.005), lower parent-reported adherence (GEC, BRI, and MI P <0.0001), lower parent-reported adolescent diabetes self-efficacy (GEC P = 0.0008, MI P <0.0001), and more parent-reported parental involvement in diabetes management (GEC P = 0.0002, BRI P = 0.003, MI P <0.0001).

Table 4

Diabetes Management Characteristics According to Parent Proxy Elevations (≥60) on the GEC, BRI, and MI

CharacteristicGECBRIMI
<60 (N = 125)≥60 (N = 43)P<60 (N = 137)≥60 (N = 31)P<60 (N = 123)≥60 (N = 45)P
Insulin pump use 74 47 0.001 74 39 0.0005 75 47 0.0008 
Blood glucose monitoring frequency, times/day 4.7 ± 2.2 3.9 ± 1.6 0.01 4.7 ± 2.2 3.8 ± 1.6 0.02 4.7 ± 2.2 4.0 ± 1.6 0.02 
A1C, % 8.4 ± 1.1 8.9 ± 1.3 0.008 8.4 ± 1.1 9.1 ± 1.4 0.002 8.3 ± 1.0 9.0 ± 1.5 0.005 
Diabetes adherence score 70 ± 13 61 ± 13 <0.0001 70 ± 13 59 ± 12 <0.0001 71 ± 13 61 ± 14 <0.0001 
Diabetes self-efficacy score 75 ± 21 62 ± 24 0.0008 74 ± 21 63 ± 25 0.01 76 ± 20 59 ± 24 <0.0001 
Diabetes parental involvement score 41 ± 13 51 ± 16 0.0002 42 ± 14 51 ± 16 0.003 41 ± 13 52 ± 15 <0.0001 
CharacteristicGECBRIMI
<60 (N = 125)≥60 (N = 43)P<60 (N = 137)≥60 (N = 31)P<60 (N = 123)≥60 (N = 45)P
Insulin pump use 74 47 0.001 74 39 0.0005 75 47 0.0008 
Blood glucose monitoring frequency, times/day 4.7 ± 2.2 3.9 ± 1.6 0.01 4.7 ± 2.2 3.8 ± 1.6 0.02 4.7 ± 2.2 4.0 ± 1.6 0.02 
A1C, % 8.4 ± 1.1 8.9 ± 1.3 0.008 8.4 ± 1.1 9.1 ± 1.4 0.002 8.3 ± 1.0 9.0 ± 1.5 0.005 
Diabetes adherence score 70 ± 13 61 ± 13 <0.0001 70 ± 13 59 ± 12 <0.0001 71 ± 13 61 ± 14 <0.0001 
Diabetes self-efficacy score 75 ± 21 62 ± 24 0.0008 74 ± 21 63 ± 25 0.01 76 ± 20 59 ± 24 <0.0001 
Diabetes parental involvement score 41 ± 13 51 ± 16 0.0002 42 ± 14 51 ± 16 0.003 41 ± 13 52 ± 15 <0.0001 

Data are % or mean ± SD.

BRI Clinical Scales

Elevated BRIEF-SR scores on the Shift and Emotional Control scales were associated with lower self-reported diabetes self-efficacy (Shift ≥60 vs. <60: 72 ± 19 vs. 84 ± 15, P = 0.004; and Emotional Control ≥60 vs. <60: 72 ± 18 vs. 83 ± 15, P = 0.004). No other relationships were statistically significant for the BRIEF-SR BRI clinical scales. Elevated BRIEF-Parent scores on the Inhibit, Shift, and Emotional Control scales were associated with lower parent proxy-reported adherence (Inhibit ≥60 vs. <60: 61 ± 13 vs. 69 ± 14, P = 0.009; Shift ≥60 vs. <60: 59 ± 13 vs. 70 ± 13, P <0.0001; and Emotional Control ≥60 vs. <60: 59 ± 11 vs. 71 ± 13, P <0.0001). Elevated BRIEF-Parent scores on the Shift scale were also associated with lower parent-reported diabetes self-efficacy (≥60 vs. <60: 62 ± 24 vs. 75 ± 21, P = 0.001).

MI Clinical Scales

Elevated BRIEF-SR scores on the Plan/Organize scale were associated with less pump use (≥60 vs. <60: 25 vs. 71%, P = 0.002) and higher A1C (≥60 vs. <60: 9.4 ± 1.3 vs. 8.4 ± 1.1%, P = 0.005). No other relationships were statistically significant for the BRIEF-SR MI clinical scales. Elevated BRIEF-Parent scores on the MI clinical scales were associated with numerous diabetes management variables (Table 5). Elevated BRIEF-Parent scores on the Initiate (P = 0.008) and Monitor (P = 0.007) scales were associated with less pump use. Elevated BRIEF-Parent scores on the Initiate scale were also associated with less frequent blood glucose monitoring (P <0.01). Elevated BRIEF-Parent scores on the Plan/Organize (P = 0.003) and Organization of Materials (P = 0.003) scales were associated with higher A1C. On all MI clinical scales, elevated BRIEF-Parent scores were associated with lower parent proxy-reported adherence (P <0.0001 to P = 0.0003) and lower parent-reported diabetes self-efficacy (P <0.0001 to P = 0.003). On all MI clinical scales except Initiate, elevated BRIEF-Parent scores were associated with more parent-reported involvement in diabetes management (P <0.0001 to P = 0.007).

Table 5

Diabetes Management Characteristics According to Parent Proxy Elevations (≥60) on the MI Clinical Scales

CharacteristicInitiateWorking MemoryPlan/OrganizeOrganization of MaterialsMonitor
<60 (N = 126)≥60 (N = 42)P<60 (N = 117)≥60 (N = 51)P<60 (N = 127)≥60 (N = 41)P<60 (N = 110)≥60 (N = 58)P<60 (N = 127)≥60 (N = 41)P
Insulin pump use 73 50 0.008 74 53 0.01 72 54 0.04 72 59 0.09 73 49 0.007 
Blood glucose monitoring frequency, times/day 4.8 ± 2.1 3.8 ± 1.8 <0.01 4.8 ± 2.2 4.0 ± 1.7 0.03 4.7 ± 2.2 3.9 ± 1.7 0.02 4.7 ± 2.2 4.2 ± 1.8 0.10 4.7 ± 2.2 4.0 ± 1.6 0.03 
A1C, % 8.4 ± 1.1 8.9 ± 1.4 0.03 8.4 ± 1.1 8.8 ± 1.4 0.04 8.4 ± 1.1 9.0 ± 1.3 0.003 8.3 ± 1.0 8.9 ± 1.4 0.003 8.4 ± 1.0 9.0 ± 1.5 0.02 
Diabetes adherence score 71 ± 12 59 ± 15 <0.0001 70 ± 13 62 ± 13 0.0003 71 ± 13 60 ± 14 <0.0001 71 ± 14 63 ± 13 0.0002 70 ± 13 61 ± 13 <0.0001 
Diabetes self-efficacy score 75 ± 21 62 ± 22 0.002 77 ± 19 60 ± 24 <0.0001 76 ± 20 58 ± 23 <0.0001 75 ± 20 65 ± 24 0.003 75 ± 20 61 ± 25 0.0005 
Diabetes parent involvement score 42 ± 14 48 ± 15 0.02 40 ± 13 52 ± 14 <0.0001 41 ± 14 51 ± 15 0.0004 41 ± 14 48 ± 14 0.007 41 ± 14 50 ± 15 0.0006 
CharacteristicInitiateWorking MemoryPlan/OrganizeOrganization of MaterialsMonitor
<60 (N = 126)≥60 (N = 42)P<60 (N = 117)≥60 (N = 51)P<60 (N = 127)≥60 (N = 41)P<60 (N = 110)≥60 (N = 58)P<60 (N = 127)≥60 (N = 41)P
Insulin pump use 73 50 0.008 74 53 0.01 72 54 0.04 72 59 0.09 73 49 0.007 
Blood glucose monitoring frequency, times/day 4.8 ± 2.1 3.8 ± 1.8 <0.01 4.8 ± 2.2 4.0 ± 1.7 0.03 4.7 ± 2.2 3.9 ± 1.7 0.02 4.7 ± 2.2 4.2 ± 1.8 0.10 4.7 ± 2.2 4.0 ± 1.6 0.03 
A1C, % 8.4 ± 1.1 8.9 ± 1.4 0.03 8.4 ± 1.1 8.8 ± 1.4 0.04 8.4 ± 1.1 9.0 ± 1.3 0.003 8.3 ± 1.0 8.9 ± 1.4 0.003 8.4 ± 1.0 9.0 ± 1.5 0.02 
Diabetes adherence score 71 ± 12 59 ± 15 <0.0001 70 ± 13 62 ± 13 0.0003 71 ± 13 60 ± 14 <0.0001 71 ± 14 63 ± 13 0.0002 70 ± 13 61 ± 13 <0.0001 
Diabetes self-efficacy score 75 ± 21 62 ± 22 0.002 77 ± 19 60 ± 24 <0.0001 76 ± 20 58 ± 23 <0.0001 75 ± 20 65 ± 24 0.003 75 ± 20 61 ± 25 0.0005 
Diabetes parent involvement score 42 ± 14 48 ± 15 0.02 40 ± 13 52 ± 14 <0.0001 41 ± 14 51 ± 15 0.0004 41 ± 14 48 ± 14 0.007 41 ± 14 50 ± 15 0.0006 

Data are % or mean ± SD.

Assessment of executive function problems in people with type 1 diabetes is important across the life span, including during adolescence. In this study, we examined adolescent self-reports and parent proxy-reports of adolescent executive function, assessed with the validated BRIEF, and associations with diabetes management characteristics and glycemic control in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Age- and sex-adjusted self-report and parent proxy-report T scores on the individual indices and clinical scales were moderately to strongly correlated. However, parent proxy-report scores were significantly higher than self-report scores. These findings are consistent with limited previous research comparing adolescent self-report and parent proxy-report BRIEF T scores in community-based samples (11,22).

Parent proxy-report scores in our sample were similar to parent proxy-report scores in normative samples (22) and other samples of children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes (5,9,12,21). The BRIEF-SR has been used less frequently in clinical research than the BRIEF-Parent, and, thus, there are fewer data for comparison. In a community-based sample of eighth-graders (aged 13–15 years), the mean BRIEF-SR GEC T score was 50.46 (22). In a study by Suchy et al. (9) involving a sample of 196 high school seniors with type 1 diabetes with a mean A1C of 8.2 ± 1.5%, the mean BRIEF-SR GEC T score was 54.11. Both of these values are higher than the self-reported GEC T score in our sample (45.09), likely reflecting the relatively narrow eligibility criteria of our sample with respect to glycemic control, given their agreement to participate in a longitudinal study aimed at improving diabetes self-care and glycemic control.

In our sample, problems with executive function were not associated with sex, age, or diabetes duration. Most of the significant associations between executive function problems and diabetes characteristics were observed for the MI clinical scales. These scales measure a person’s abilities in areas such as planning and initiating an activity, generating problem-solving strategies, holding information in working memory, and maintaining organization in their environment. Adolescents with type 1 diabetes who have problems with metacognition may have challenges with diabetes management tasks (e.g., keeping track of supplies and checking glucose levels). They may also have difficulties gathering the information needed to calculate an insulin dose (e.g., glucose level, carbohydrate amount in meal/snack, and planned activity) or troubleshooting problems with diabetes devices such as insulin pumps or CGM systems. Problems with executive function may lead parents to avoid insulin pump use because pump use tends to place more self-care responsibility in adolescents’ hands.

The relationships between executive function and adherence and glycemic control in our sample are in line with previous research (68). Not all studies have demonstrated a significant relationship between executive function and glycemic control. However, A1C may be related to executive function through the mediating variable of adherence, as demonstrated by McNally et al. (12). Interestingly, some studies have shown significant relationships between executive function and survey-based measures of adherence but not blood glucose monitoring frequency (5,21). In our study, only the BRIEF-Parent Initiate scale was significantly associated with blood glucose monitoring frequency.

Diabetes management is complex and involves not only checking glucose levels, but also estimating carbohydrate intake, determining insulin doses, and administering insulin, while considering factors such as exercise and acute illnesses. Problems with executive function may negatively affect these many areas of diabetes management. For example, in qualitative semistructured interviews with six adolescents who had type 1 diabetes and ADHD, participants reported problems with establishing and maintaining diabetes management routines and reported that they would like family and school personnel to be more involved in helping them with tasks such as interpreting glucose levels and insulin dose calculations (23). Even as diabetes monitoring and treatment technologies become more automated, executive function is still necessary to integrate information, make treatment decisions, maintain supplies, and troubleshoot technology failures.

Our primary aim was to assess associations between diabetes characteristics and specific components of executive function assessed by individual clinical scales. We found stronger associations for the MI scales than for the BRI scales. In contrast, Miller et al. (24) found that poorer behavioral regulation was associated with poorer adherence over 2 years but did not find an association between metacognition and adherence. However, the study participants were younger (baseline ages 9–11 years) than the participants in our study and may have had more parental involvement in diabetes management, which likely helped overcome the impact of metacognitive challenges. In a longitudinal study by Suchy et al. (25), both behavioral and cognitive aspects of executive function were related to glycemic control during the transition to young adulthood. Osipoff et al. (26) also found an association between metacognitive executive function problems, by parent proxy-report, and higher A1C in youth with type 1 diabetes aged 6–18 years.

Although the BRIEF is one of the most commonly used rating scales for assessing executive function, there are limited published data providing scores and rates of elevations for all components of the BRIEF (i.e., the GEC, BRI, MI, and clinical scales) in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. This article adds valuable new information to the scientific literature by providing such detailed descriptive data for both parent proxy-report and adolescent self-report in a contemporary sample of adolescents with type 1 diabetes. In assessments of adolescent functioning, self-report and parent proxy-report scores are often correlated but may differ in magnitude (27). Thus, the perspectives of multiple raters can provide valuable information when using survey-based assessment tools such as the BRIEF. Indeed, a recent review article on executive function in adolescents and young adults with type 1 diabetes recommended that “future studies incorporate both self and caregiver reports in measurement . . . . Inclusion of adolescents’ and young adults’ perspective in research and clinical care demonstrate values of their voice in medical decision-making” (28).

The percentage of adolescent/parent dyads in which there was discordance resulting from an elevated BRIEF-Parent score without an elevated BRIEF-SR score ranged from 12–27%, with slightly higher levels of discordance on the MI scales than on the BRI scales. One possible explanation for the differences in scores is social desirability bias. Adolescents may underestimate problems, either consciously or unconsciously, whereas parents may be more willing to rate their adolescent’s behaviors as occurring “sometimes” or “often” (vs. “never”). Adolescent and parent perceptions regarding frequency may also differ. Adolescents may perceive that forgetting to bring home school assignments once a week is “sometimes,” whereas parents may perceive this to be “often.”

One limitation of the study is the relative homogeneity of the study sample. The majority of participants were non-Hispanic White, and approximately two-thirds used an insulin pump. Additional research is needed in more diverse samples. Indeed, the observation of higher rates of executive function problems in racial/ethnic minorities by parent proxy-report may reflect type 1 error, highlighting the need for further study in larger samples. Approximately one-fifth of participants were using CGM, and analyses did not reveal any differences in CGM use according to executive function problems; further research regarding associations of executive function problems and CGM use in other samples with more CGM use will be informative.

Another limitation is that we did not check participants’ blood glucose at the time of survey completion to ensure no hypoglycemia or hyperglycemia. We also did not collect data from the medical record regarding diagnosis or management of ADHD, although 14% of the sample had ADHD by self-report.

Finally, the study sample was limited to those who met eligibility criteria for the longitudinal study. The sample did not include adolescents with an A1C >11.0%, significant developmental or cognitive disorders, or significant mental illness, as these factors may have affected individuals’ ability to fully participate in the longitudinal study. Future research on executive function can include these youth. Studies have shown that chronic hyperglycemia has a detrimental impact on cognitive function in children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes (29), and rates of executive function problems may have been even higher if individuals with an A1C >11.0% were included in the sample.

Although it is likely not practical to implement the BRIEF as a routine screening measure in clinical settings, diabetes health care providers may be aware of potential difficulties with executive function from other sources such as a diagnosis of ADHD, school-based screening tests, or reports from other health care providers. Providers can also ask adolescents and their parents about difficulties with diabetes management that may signal executive function problems.

If providers do have access to the results of screening measures such as the BRIEF that provide information about specific domains of executive function problems, this information can be particularly helpful in targeting specific areas for intervention. At times, adolescents can be referred for specific testing with trained mental health professionals. When there is information to suggest problems with executive function, providers should work with the adolescent, family, and, possibly, school nurse to provide the additional support needed. This strategy may include increasing parental involvement in the adolescent’s diabetes self-management, scheduling more frequent clinic appointments, and/or tailoring the diabetes treatment plan to better match the adolescent’s understanding and capabilities. Providers can also help to develop strategies to mitigate difficulties with specific aspects of diabetes management (e.g., checking glucose levels and remembering to carry diabetes supplies). Finally, providers should work with the family and school personnel to ensure that the adolescent has appropriate support at school (e.g., a 504 Plan).

In summary, adequate executive function is important for success in daily tasks. As adolescents take on more responsibilities and become more independent in multiple areas of their life (e.g., school, social activities, work, and health care) and prepare for transitions away from the family to college or the workplace and from pediatric to adult health care settings, executive function plays an important role in whether they will be successful in assuming additional responsibilities and navigating such transitions. In this study, we found that problems with executive function, primarily for the MI clinical scales, were associated with poorer adherence and poorer glycemic control. Adolescents with type 1 diabetes who have problems with metacognition may need additional support from parents, other family members, and health care providers to carry out diabetes management tasks. Thus, identification of executive function problems in adolescents with type 1 diabetes is important, since it can provide an area for active intervention. Further research is needed to elucidate the differences in adolescent self-reports and parent proxy-reports of executive function problems using the BRIEF.

M.H. and L.K.V. contributed equally as co-first authors.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (grant numbers R01DK095273 and P30DK036836), JDRF (grant number 2-SRA-2014-253-M-B), the Katherine Adler Astrove Youth Education Fund, the Maria Griffin Drury Pediatric Fund, and the Eleanor Chesterman Beatson Fund. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of these organizations.

Duality of Interest

No potential conflicts of interest relevant to this article were reported.

Author Contributions

M.H. performed data analyses and data interpretation and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. L.K.V. designed the study; collected, analyzed, and interpreted data; and participated in the manuscript writing and revision. R.K.S. and Z.G. performed data collection and interpretation. L.M.L. developed the study conception and design, performed data collection and interpretation, and participated in manuscript revisions. L.M.L. is the guarantor of this work and, as such, had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Prior Presentation

Portions of this article were presented at the American Diabetes Association’s 76th Scientific Sessions in New Orleans, LA, 10–14 June 2016, and 78th Scientific Sessions in Orlando, FL, 22–26 June 2018.

1.
Goldstein
S
,
Naglieri
JA
,
Princiotta
D
,
Otero
TM
.
Introduction: a history of executive functioning as a theoretical and clinical construct
. In
Handbook of Executive Functioning
.
Goldstein
S
,
Naglieri
JA
, Eds.
New York
,
Springer Science+Business Media
,
2014
, p.
3
12
2.
Suchy
Y
.
Executive functioning: overview, assessment, and research issues for non-neuropsychologists
.
Ann Behav Med
2009
;
37
:
106
116
3.
Romine
CB
,
Reynolds
CR
.
A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: findings from a meta-analysis
.
Appl Neuropsychol
2005
;
12
:
190
201
4.
Wasserman
RM
,
Anderson
BJ
,
Schwartz
DD
.
Screening of neurocognitive and executive functioning in children, adolescents, and young adults with type 1 diabetes
.
Diabetes Spectr
2016
;
29
:
202
210
5.
Perez
KM
,
Patel
NJ
,
Lord
JH
, et al
.
Executive function in adolescents with type 1 diabetes: relationship to adherence, glycemic control, and psychosocial outcomes
.
J Pediatr Psychol
2017
;
42
:
636
646
6.
Goethals
ER
,
de Wit
M
,
Van Broeck
N
, et al
.
Child and parental executive functioning in type 1 diabetes: their unique and interactive role toward treatment adherence and glycemic control
.
Pediatr Diabetes
2018
;
19
:
520
526
7.
Nylander
C
,
Tindberg
Y
,
Haas
J
, et al
.
Self- and parent-reported executive problems in adolescents with type 1 diabetes are associated with poor metabolic control and low physical activity
.
Pediatr Diabetes
2018
;
19
:
98
105
8.
Bagner
DM
,
Williams
LB
,
Geffken
GR
,
Silverstein
JH
,
Storch
EA
.
Type 1 diabetes in youth: the relationship between adherence and executive functioning
.
Child Health Care
2007
;
36
:
169
179
9.
Suchy
Y
,
Turner
SL
,
Queen
TL
, et al
.
The relation of questionnaire and performance-based measures of executive functioning with type 1 diabetes outcomes among late adolescents
.
Health Psychol
2016
;
35
:
661
669
10.
Roth
RM
,
Isquith
PK
,
Gioia
GA
.
Assessment of executive functioning using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)
. In
Handbook of Executive Functioning
.
Goldstein
S
,
Naglieri
JA
, Eds.
New York
,
Springer Science+Business Media
,
2014
, p.
301
331
11.
Guy
SC
,
Isquith
PK
,
Gioia
GA
.
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function–Self-Report Version: Professional Manual
.
Lutz, FL
,
PAR
,
2004
12.
McNally
K
,
Rohan
J
,
Pendley
JS
,
Delamater
A
,
Drotar
D
.
Executive functioning, treatment adherence, and glycemic control in children with type 1 diabetes
.
Diabetes Care
2010
;
33
:
1159
1162
13.
Vloemans
AF
,
Eilander
MMA
,
Rotteveel
J
, et al
.
Youth with type 1 diabetes taking responsibility for self-management: the importance of executive functioning in achieving glycemic control: results from the longitudinal DINO study
.
Diabetes Care
2019
;
42
:
225
231
14.
Berg
CA
,
Wiebe
DJ
,
Suchy
Y
, et al
.
Individual differences and day-to-day fluctuations in perceived self-regulation associated with daily adherence in late adolescents with type 1 diabetes
.
J Pediatr Psychol
2014
;
39
:
1038
1048
15.
McGill
DE
,
Laffel
LM
,
Volkening
LK
, et al
.
Text message intervention for teens with type 1 diabetes preserves HbA1c: results of a randomized controlled trial
.
Diabetes Technol Ther
2020
;
22
:
374
382
16.
Harris
PA
,
Taylor
R
,
Thielke
R
,
Payne
J
,
Gonzalez
N
,
Conde
JG
.
Research electronic data capture (REDCap): a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support
.
J Biomed Inform
2009
;
42
:
377
381
17.
Mehta
SN
,
Nansel
TR
,
Volkening
LK
,
Butler
DA
,
Haynie
DL
,
Laffel
LM
.
Validation of a contemporary adherence measure for children with type 1 diabetes: the Diabetes Management Questionnaire
.
Diabet Med
2015
;
32
:
1232
1238
18.
Giani
E
,
Commissariat
PV
,
Wasserman
RM
,
Volkening
LK
,
Laffel
LM
,
Anderson
BJ
.
Validation of the Diabetes Management Self-Efficacy Survey for teens with T1D and their parents [Abstract]
.
Diabetes
2016
;
65
:
A209
19.
Anderson
BJ
,
Auslander
WF
,
Jung
KC
,
Miller
JP
,
Santiago
JV
.
Assessing family sharing of diabetes responsibilities
.
J Pediatr Psychol
1990
;
15
:
477
492
20.
Kenny
DA
,
Kashy
DA
,
Cook
WL
.
Dyadic Data Analysis
.
New York
,
Guilford Press
,
2006
21.
Hamburger
ER
,
Lyttle
M
,
Compas
BE
,
Jaser
SS
.
Performance-based and questionnaire measures of executive function in adolescents with type 1 diabetes
.
J Behav Med
2019
;
42
:
1041
1049
22.
Egan
KN
,
Cohen
LA
,
Limbers
C
.
Parent-child agreement on the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning (BRIEF) in a community sample of adolescents
.
Appl Neuropsychol Child
2019
;
8
:
264
271
23.
Lindblad
I
,
Engström
AC
,
Nylander
C
,
Fernell
E
.
Adolescents with type 1 diabetes mellitus and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder require specific support from healthcare professionals
.
Acta Paediatr
2017
;
106
:
1994
1997
24.
Miller
MM
,
Rohan
JM
,
Delamater
A
, et al
.
Changes in executive functioning and self-management in adolescents with type 1 diabetes: a growth curve analysis
.
J Pediatr Psychol
2013
;
38
:
18
29
25.
Suchy
Y
,
Butner
J
,
Wiebe
DJ
,
Campbell
M
,
Turner
SL
,
Berg
CA
.
Executive cognitive functions and behavioral control differentially predict HbA1c in type 1 diabetes across emerging adulthood
.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc
2020
;
26
:
353
363
26.
Osipoff
JN
,
Dixon
D
,
Wilson
TA
,
Preston
T
.
Prospective memory and glycemic control in children with type 1 diabetes mellitus: a cross-sectional study
.
Int J Pediatr Endocrinol
2012
;
2012
:
29
27.
Yi-Frazier
JP
,
Hilliard
ME
,
Fino
NF
, et al
.
Whose quality of life is it anyway? Discrepancies between youth and parent health-related quality of life ratings in type 1 and type 2 diabetes
.
Qual Life Res
2016
;
25
:
1113
1121
28.
Ding
K
,
Reynolds
CM
,
Driscoll
KA
,
Janicke
DM
.
The relationship between executive functioning, type 1 diabetes self-management behaviors, and glycemic control in adolescents and young adults
.
Curr Diab Rep
2021
;
21
:
10
29.
Nevo-Shenker
M
,
Shalitin
S
.
The impact of hypo- and hyperglycemia on cognition and brain development in young children with type 1 diabetes
.
Horm Res Paediatr
2021
;
94
:
115
123
Readers may use this article as long as the work is properly cited, the use is educational and not for profit, and the work is not altered. More information is available at https://www.diabetesjournals.org/journals/pages/license.